Available online at www.sciencedirect.com
ScienceDirect
Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 (2014) 665 - 670
WCPCG 2014
Personal Need for Structure in Relation to Language Variables
Stranovskä Evaa*, Hvozdikovä Silviab, Munkovä Däsac
aDepartment of German Studies, Constantine the Philosopher University, Stefânikova 67, Nitra, 949 74 Slovakia bDepartment of English and Americans Studies, Constantine the Philosopher University, Stefânikova 67, Nitra, 949 74 Slovakia cDepartment of Translatology,Constantine the Philosopher University, Stefânikova 67, Nitra, 949 74 Slovakia
Abstract
The present study focuses on determination of relationship between personal need for structure PNS, desire for structure F1 and response to lack of structure F2, and language variables, namely, reading comprehension in foreign language and language proficiency. The research focussed at personal need for structure in context of learning a foreign language. Specifically, whether comprehension of foreign texts is connected to desire for structure or it is connected to response to lack of structure. The results of the experiment showed negative correlation between personal need for structure and reading comprehension in foreign language and also between personal need for structure and language proficiency. © 2014 Publishedby ElsevierLtd.Thisis anopen access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.Org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of the Academic World Education and Research Center. Keywords: Personal Need for Structure; Foreign Language Reading Comprehension; Foreign Language;
1. Introduction
The issue of Personal Need for Structure have been examined in academic setting mostly in terms of relations between personal need for structure and stereotypes, bias, ambiguity, tolerance, intolerance on ambiguity, anxiety or rigidity (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993; Sarmany-Schuller, 1999; Kruglanski & Webster, 1996, 200; Stangor, 2000; Thomson at al., 2001 and others). However, personal need for structure has not yet been explored in context of linguistic variables, specifically, the relationship to the skill of reading comprehension. Stranovska et al. (2013) proved negative relationship between need for structure and verbal intelligence (Stranovska at al. 2013). Reading comprehension in foreign language is affected by language and cognitive processes. Ehlers (1998) claims there are not research studies explaining effect of linguistic and cognitive variables on reading comprehension in foreign
* Silvia Hvozdikova. Tel.: +67 949 74. E-mail address: silvia.hvozdikova@ukf.sk
1877-0428 © 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of the Academic World Education and Research Center. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.458
language. The focus of the current study was to find out relationship between the skill of reading comprehension in foreign language and need for structure as an individual's categorisation activity.
2. Need for Structure and Reading Comprehension in Foreign Language
Cognitive individual variable Personal Need for Structure is characterised by representation of simplified information, generalising of previous experience (Markus & Zajonc, 1985; Allport, 1954), organising information into less complex categories used in new and ambiguous situations to preserve certainty (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993). The research of personal need for structure is mainly associated with the following variables: stereotypes and bias (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993; Sarmany-Schuller, 1999; Stangor, 2000). High need for structure is related to the need for rapid, simple and exact responses and for diverting from uncertain or ambiguous information (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996, 2000; Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). Such and individual feels uncomfortable and uncertain in uncertain situations (Thompson, et al., 2001; Neuber, et al., 1997; Neuberg & Newsom, 1993). Neuberg and Newsom (1993) identified two conceptual different factors of need for structure: 1. Desire for structure, F1, and 2. Response to lack of structure, F2. Desire for structure, F1 refers to the extent of desired structure of individuals in daily lives. Response to lack of structure, F2, refers to individuals' responses to unstructured, unpredictable situations. Individuals who expressively dislike uncertain situations or changes in their plans at the last moment achieve high scores in responses to lack of structure. Psycholinguistic model of Goodman (1967, 1971) and Smith (1971) has a determining influence on the research of the linguistic variable, foreign-language reading comprehension in cognitive psychology and linguistics. Goodman universal hypothesis reading in mother tongue and in foreign language are identical processes stimulate researchers to verify the following hypotheses: 1. Interdependency hypothesis: Reading comprehension in foreign language depends on cognitive skill of reading in mother tongue (Bernhard, 1991; Coady, 1979; Hudson, 1982). 2. Linguistic threshold hypothesis: Reading comprehension in foreign language depends on language competence in that language (Clarke, 1980; Cziko, 1978). Clarke (1980) found out that a reader with a high or a low linguistic competence uses the same graphic, syntactical and semantic indicators in foreign language text as they use in mother tongue. According to Cziko (1978) only advanced reader in mother tongue as well as in foreign language is able to recognise semantic and pragmatic indicators in text. Furthermore, beginners in learning of foreign language are focused on graphic aspects in reading whereas advanced students use reading strategies of mother tongue and extra textual indicators. Hudson (1982) built on Clarke's research studies (1979, 1980) and shares the same view of the positive influence of foreign language proficiency on reading, but on the other hand he distinguishes reader's performance in mother tongue. It is important for a reader not only to activate the received knowledge but also to use it and to connect it with new knowledge. Gadusova (2004) studies also emotional indicators, imagination, cultural specifications and motivation. The latter research studies are focused on exploration of predictors of reading, mainly on a working memory, language sensitivity, rapid naming, phonological awareness (Foy & Mann, 2006; Anthony & Francis, 2005; Smith-Spark & Fisk, 2007).
3. Research Methodology
3.1. Research sample
Research was carried out at Constantine the Philosopher University in Nitra in the academic years 2011/2012, 2012/2013 and 2013/2014. It was attended by 221 students enrolled in various faculties and programmes, studying foreign language as their minors, namely, English/German for Academic Purposes. They were full-time students in the second year. The average age of the students was 20,5 years. The average number of years of learning English was 9 years and German 8 years.
3.2. Hypotheses
The following hypothesis was assumed: HO There is a negative relationship of need for structure and reading comprehension in foreign language. The correlation analysis served as a tool for testing the hypothesis.
3.3. Research methods and tests of language proficiency
PNS Scale (Thomson at al., 2001) - Personal Need for Structure is derived from the assumption that the ability to reduce the uncertainty of situation and cognitive burden or to manage simplified information is connected with managing unknown situations. The scale consists of 12 items. The Slovak version of the PNS Scale was used in the study (Sarmany-Schuller, 1999). The PNS construct is based on two-factors concept of personal need for structure: 1. Desire for structure (sub-factor Fl) and 2. Response to lack of structure (sub-factor F2). Test of foreign language proficiency in both languages, English and German. The test consists of 74 items and measures individual ability in morphology, syntax, lexicology, cultural studies, and reading comprehension. The tests were designed according to ISED 3 level B1/B2.
3.4. Results
Table 1 displays statistically significant correlations among examined variables - Fl, F2, PNS, reading comprehension, language proficiency and years of study. Relations of examined variables are investigated for English and German separately. For English language, the coefficients of correlation matric show statistically significant positive relationship between foreign language proficiency and reading comprehension in foreign language (r= 0.571) and statistically significant negative relationship between desire for structure and years of learning English (r= -0.190). For German language, the coefficients of the correlation matric displays statistically significant positive relationship between foreign language proficiency and reading comprehension in foreign language (r= 0.779) and statistically significant negative relationship between desire for structure and foreign language proficiency (r= -0.208).
Table 1. Correlate matric of examined variables - English language
Years of FL study Language test score Reading comprehension test score F1 F2 PNS
Years of FL study 1.000 -0.059 -0.028 -0.190 -0.075 -0.148
Language test score -0.059 1.000 0.571 0.049 0.041 0.053
Reading comprehension test score -0.028 0.571 1.000 0.056 0.047 0.061
F1 -0.190 0.049 0.056 1.000 0.389 0.773
F2 -0.075 0.041 0.047 0.389 1.000 0.885
PNS -0.148 0.053 0.061 0.773 0.885 1.000
Table 2. Correlate matric of examined variables - German language
Years of FL study Language test s core Reading comprehension test score F1 F2 PNS
Years of FL study 1.000 0.002 0.064 -0.105 -0.071 -0.098
Language test score 0.002 1.000 0.779 -0.208 -0.049 -0.132
Reading comprehension 0.064 0.779 1.000 -0.178 -0.066 -0.129
test score
F1 -0.105 -0.208 -0.178 1.000 0.490 0.807
F2 -0.071 -0.049 -0.066 0.490 1.000 0.910
PNS -0.098 -0.132 -0.129 0.807 0.910 1.000
The relationships among the examined variables according to field of study - faculty affiliations: ff- Faculty of Arts, fpv- Faculty of Natural Sciences were investigated.
Table 4. Correlate matric of examined variables - Faculty of Natural Sciences
Years of FL study Language test s core Reading_comprehension_ test_score F1 F2 PNS
Years of FL study 1.000 -0.104 -0.009 -0.290 -0.140 -0.252
Language test score -0.104 1.000 0.565 0.230 0.174 0.241
Reading comprehension test _score -0.009 0.565 1.000 0.193 -0.025 0.089
F1 -0.290 0.230 0.193 1.000 0.368 0.793
F2 -0.140 0.174 -0.025 0.368 1.000 0.858
PNS -0.252 0.241 0.089 0.793 0.858 1.000
Table 5. Correlate matric of examined variables - Faculty of Arts
Years_of_FL_study Language_test_ score Reading_comprehension_test _score F1 F2 PNS
Years_of_FL_study 1.000 0.059 0.058 0.064 0.107 0.101
Language_test_score 0.059 1.000 0.906 -0.452 0.051 -0.172
Reading_comprehension_test _score 0.058 0.906 1.000 -0.455 -0.055 -0.244
F1 0.064 -0.452 -0.455 1.000 0.579 0.841
F2 0.107 0.051 -0.055 0.579 1.000 0.928
PNS 0.101 -0.172 -0.244 0.841 0.928 1.000
In the category - fpv- Faculty of Natural Sciences, there was statistical significance showed between need for structure and reading comprehension in foreign language or the period of foreign language learning. However, statistically significant positive relationship between foreign language proficiency and reading comprehension in a foreign language (r= 0.565) was found out. In the category - ff- Faculty of Arts, statistically significant negative relationship between desire for structure and foreign language proficiency (r= -0.452), statistically significant negative relationship between desire for structure and reading comprehension in foreign language (r= -0.452) and statistically significant positive relationship between foreign language proficiency and reading comprehension in a foreign language (r= 0.906) were found out.
4. Discussion and Conclusion
The assumption of negative relationship between need for structure and reading comprehension in foreign language was confirmed in the case of sub-factor F1: desire for structure. There is a negative correlation between reading comprehension in foreign language and desire for structure of students of the faculty of arts, i.e. the lower
the desire for structure, the higher the skill of reading comprehension and vice versa. Apparently, desire for structure is the predictor for reading comprehension in foreign language by the students of the faculty of arts. The students use more complex information from the text, they avoid simplification. They have less cognitive burden by managing information from a foreign text. They seek semantic, pragmatic and extra textual indicators in text, specifically cultural specifications, connotations, etc. They do not focus on rules, structure or patterns of sentences or denotations. Desire for structure is associated with need for structured and organised information. Moreover, statistically significant negative correlation was proved only in the case of students of the faculty of arts. There were no significant correlations found out between desire for structure and reading comprehension by the students of the other faculties. However, there is no statistically significant difference among faculties in terms of the score of the variable desire for structure. Based on the study reading comprehension positively correlates with foreign language proficiency, i.e., the higher foreign language proficiency, the higher reading comprehension. The results confirmed the linguistic threshold hypothesis as Clarke (1980), Cziko (1978), Eskey (1973) stated. Foreign language students need to firstly receive certain degree of language competence in the studied language to be capable to comprehend the text in foreign language. Any further research would examine the need for structure in relation to language variables of the students studying foreign languages as their major programmes.
Acknowledgements
The research study was supported by the Slovak Research and Development Agency under the contract no. APVV-0451-10.
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice Reading. MA: Addison-Wesley.
Anthony, J. L. Francis, D. J.,( 2005). Development of Phonological Awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 5, 255-259. Carrell, P. L. (1983). Three components of background knowledge in reading comprehension. Language Learning. Applied Linguistics, 33, 2, 183-207.
Clarke, M. A. (1980). The Short Circuit Hypothesis of ESL Reading - or When Language Competence Interferes with Reading. The Modern Language Journal, 64, 1, 203-210.
Coady, J.(1979). A psycholinguistic model of the ESL reader. In: R. Mackay, B. Barkman, & R. R. Jordan (Eds.), Reading in a second
language, Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 5-12. Cziko, G. A. (1978). Differences in first- and second-language reading: The use of syntactic, semantic and discourse constraints. Canadian
Modern Language Review, 34, 479-489. Foy, J. G., Mann, V. (2006). Changes in Letter Sound Knowledge are Associated with Development of Phonological Awareness in Pre-School
Children. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 2, 143-161. Gadusova, 2004, Teoria aprax osvojovania cudzich jazykov, Prijemca: Diet'a. Nitra: Univerzita Konstantina Filozofa - Filozoficka fakulta, 8789.
Goodman, K. (1967). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. Journal of the Reading Specialist, 6, 126-135.
Goodman, K S. (1971). Psycholinguistic universals in the reading process. In: P. Pimsleur, T. Quinn (eds.), The Psychology of Second
Language Learning, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 135-142. Hudson, T., 1982, The effects of induced schemata on the "short circuit" in L2 reading: Non-decoding factors in L2 reading performance.
Language Learning, 32, 1-31.
Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash N. N., Pierro, A., Atash N. N., Pierro, A., Shah, J. Y., Spiegel, S. (2000). To "do the right thing" or to "just do it": Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 793-815.
Kruglanski, A. W., Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: "Seizing" and "freezing". Psychological Review, 103, 263-283. Markus, H., Zajonc, R. B. (1985). The cognitive perspective in social psychology. In: G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social
psychology (137-230). New York: Random House. Neuberg, S. L., Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differencies in the desire for simple structure. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 113-131. Neuberg, S. L., West, S. G., Judice, T. N., Thompson, M. M. (1997). On dimensionality, discriminant validity, and the role of psychometric analyses in personality theory and measurement: Reply to Kruglanski et al.'s (1997) defence of the Need for Closure Scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 5, 1017-1029. Sarmany-Schuller, I.(1999). Neistota - istota / pripad kauzalnej neistoty. In: I. Sarmany- Schuller, M. Bratska, E. Nanistova (Eds.), Reflexie
sucasnejpsychologie na Slovensku, Zbornikprispevkov IX. zjazdu slovenskych psychologov, Bratislava: STIMUL. Sarmany-Schuller, I., 1999, Procrastination, Need for Cognition and Sensation Seeking. Studiapsychologica, 41, 1, 73-85.
Smith-Spark, J. H., Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15, 1, 34-56. Stangor, C., 2000, Stereotypes and Prejudice. New York: Psychology Press.
Stranovska, E., Munkova, D., Munk, M., Sarmany-Schuller, I. (2013). Cognitive-Individual, Linguistic and Demographic Variables, and
Syntactic Abilities in Foreign Language. Studia Psychologica, 55, 4, 273-287. Thompson, M. M., Naccarato, M. E., Parker, K. C. H., Moskowitz, G. (2001). The Personal Need for Structure (PNS) and Personal Fear of Invalidity (PFI) scales: Historical perspectives, present applications and future directions. In: G. Moskowitz (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.