Scholarly article on topic 'Moving into the wide clinical spectrum of consciousness disorders: Pearls, perils and pitfalls'

Moving into the wide clinical spectrum of consciousness disorders: Pearls, perils and pitfalls Academic research paper on "Clinical medicine"

CC BY-NC-ND
0
0
Share paper
Academic journal
Medicina
OECD Field of science
Keywords
{Consciousness / Sleep / Coma / "Vegetative state" / "Minimally conscious state"}

Abstract of research paper on Clinical medicine, author of scientific article — Rocco Salvatore Calabrò, Demetrio Milardi, Alberto Cacciola, Angela Marra, Giuseppina Digangi, et al.

Abstract The last few years have been characterized by a growing interest of the medical and scientific world for the field of consciousness and its related disorders. Medically speaking, consciousness can be defined as the state of awareness of self and environment and the alertness to external stimulation, besides responsiveness to inner need. Transient loss of consciousness can be due to alterations in cerebral blood flow leading to fainting or syncope, migraine, metabolic dysfunctions, unexpected intracranial pressure increases, epileptic seizures, and sleep disorders. Chronic disorders of consciousness are a tragic success of high-technology treatment, in an attempt to maintain or reestablish brain function, which is to be considered as the main goal of therapeutics. Management of vegetative or a minimally conscious state individuals involves charily getting the right diagnosis with an evidence-based prognosis, also taking into account the medical, ethical, and legal key factors of the ideal treatment. This paper is aimed at exploring the wide spectrum of consciousness disorders and their clinical differential diagnosis, with particular regards to those with a negative impact on patient and their caregiver quality of life, including epilepsy, sleep disorders, and vegetative/minimally conscious state.

Academic research paper on topic "Moving into the wide clinical spectrum of consciousness disorders: Pearls, perils and pitfalls"

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

journal homepage http://www.elsevier.com/locate/medici

Review

Moving into the wide clinical spectrum of consciousness disorders: Pearls, perils and pitfalls

Rocco Salvatore Calabroa*, Demetrio Milardia,b, Alberto Cacciolaa,b, Angela Marra' Giuseppina Digangia, Carmela Casellac, Alfredo Manulia, Rosaria De Luca a, Rosalia Silvestric, Placido Bramantia

a IRCCS Neurolesi "Bomno Pulejo'' Center, Messina, Italy

b Department of Biomedical Sciences and Morphological and Functional Images, University of Messina, Messina, Italy c Neurologic Unit, University Polyclinic, Messina, Italy

ARTICLE INFO

ABSTRACT

Article history: Received 20 March 2015 Received in revised form 21 November 2015 Accepted 6 January 2016 Available online 15 January 2016

Keywords: Consciousness Sleep Coma

Vegetative state Minimally conscious state

The last few years have been characterized by a growing interest of the medical and scientific world for the field of consciousness and its related disorders. Medically speaking, consciousness can be defined as the state of awareness of self and environment and the alertness to external stimulation, besides responsiveness to inner need.

Transient loss of consciousness can be due to alterations in cerebral blood flow leading to fainting or syncope, migraine, metabolic dysfunctions, unexpected intracranial pressure increases, epileptic seizures, and sleep disorders. Chronic disorders of consciousness are a tragic success of high-technology treatment, in an attempt to maintain or reestablish brain function, which is to be considered as the main goal of therapeutics. Management of vegetative or a minimally conscious state individuals involves charily getting the right diagnosis with an evidence-based prognosis, also taking into account the medical, ethical, and legal key factors of the ideal treatment. This paper is aimed at exploring the wide spectrum of consciousness disorders and their clinical differential diagnosis, with particular regards to those with a negative impact on patient and their caregiver quality of life, including epilepsy, sleep disorders, and vegetative/minimally conscious state.

# 2016 The Lithuanian University of Health Sciences. Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.

org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

* Corresponding author at: IRCCS Neurolesi ''Bonino Pulejo'' Center, S.S. 113, Via Palermo, C. da Casazza, 98124 Messina, Italy. Tel.: +39 09060128840; fax: +39 09060128850.

E-mail address: salbro77@tiscali.it (R.S. Calabro). Peer review under the responsibility of the Lithuanian University of Health Sciences.

http://dx.doi.Org/10.1016/j.medici.2016.01.002

1010-660X/© 2016 The Lithuanian University of Health Sciences. Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Introduction

The terms consciousness, confusion, stupor, unconsciousness, and coma have been given many different meanings so that it is really difficult to avoid ambiguity in their use. Medically speaking, consciousness can be defined as the state of awareness of self and environment and the alertness to external stimulation, besides responsiveness to inner need [1]. Advances in neurobiology of consciousness lead to believe that the upper brainstem nuclei have a pivotal role in arousal, and that the activity of the thalamus and cortex provides much of the content of consciousness [2,3].

In particular, consciousness has 2 clinical elements:

(1) wakefulness, which is mediated by the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) of the brainstem and its thalamo-cortical connections, and

(2) awareness of one's self and the environment, which is mediated by the cerebral cortex and its intracortical, thalamo-cortical, and cortico-sub-cortical connections [4].

Alterations one or both of these components may lead to disordered state of consciousness [5]. Disordered states of consciousness define a broad category encompassing a spectrum of cognitive abnormalities, i.e. from mild confusion-al states, delirium and dementia to coma, vegetative state (VS), minimally conscious state (MCS), and brain death (Figure).

2. Episodic impairment of consciousness

Transient loss of consciousness can be due to alterations in cerebral blood flow leading to fainting or syncope, migraine, metabolic dysfunctions, unexpected intracranial pressure increases, cerebral ischemia or hemorrhage, epileptic seizures, and sleep disorders [6]. Differentiating anxiety attacks, psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES), and malingering from the abovementioned conditions may be really difficult.

Figure - The key elements of consciousness, i.e. wakefulness and awareness of environment and self that are positively correlated in the physiological states. A complete dissociation between such elements is clearly seen in vegetative state. Notably, individuals suffering from coma are unconscious and cannot be awakened.

Notably, the right diagnosis may not be reached without accurate laboratory tests and long follow-up periods.

2.1. Syncope

Syncope may be caused by a reduced cardiac output owing to heart arrhythmias, outflow obstacle, orthostatic hypotension, hypovolemia or reduced venous return. Metabolic dysfunction, caused by hypoxia, anemia, hypoglycemia or drugs, may usually lead to presyncope (i.e. a sensation of an imminent faint) or, less commonly, to syncope. Of note, consciousness maybe impaired in absence seizures, generalized tonic-clonic seizures (GTCS), and complex partial seizures that, in most cases, can be easily distinguished from syncope.

However, the differential diagnosis may be very difficult when syncope is associated to myoclonic jerks, upward version of the eyes and short automatism [7-11].

2.2. Epileptic seizures

Epileptic seizures (ES) are the manifestation of cortical neuron abnormal hypersynchronization with a consequent hyperex-citable discharges. Physiologically, ES has been defined as an abrupt alteration of brain function, subsequent to a paroxysmal high-frequency and high voltage electrical discharge, mainly arising from an assemblage of excitable neurons in any part of the cerebral cortex [12,13]. It is noteworthy that seizures are frequent, aspecific expressions of neurologic disorders, including brain injury, since the leading function of the brain is the transmission of electrical impulses.

Epilepsy is defined as a brain disease characterized by a durable tendency to produce epileptic seizures with the consequent neurobiological, neuropsychological, and psychosocial complications. The diagnosis of epilepsy usually requires the existence of at least 2 unprovoked seizures 24 h apart [14]. Seizures are divided into two categories: partial seizures and generalized seizures. Partial seizures result from a neural discharge within a specific brain area, with focal symptoms that may progress (with or without consciousness impairment) to secondarily generalized seizure, resulting in tonic-clonic activity [15].

Primary generalized seizures are thought to be generated in the thalamus and other related subcortical areas, although EEG recording usually shows a simultaneous activation of both the cerebral hemispheres. Thus, they present with bilateral symptoms and/or signs, and are constantly associated to loss of consciousness. Tonic seizure is the rigid (usually brief) contracture of muscles, whereas the clonic seizure is the longer rhythmic shaking; the consequent GTCS, also defined "a grand mal'', may be considered one of the most dramatic medical illnesses. Patients affected by GTCS and idiopathic generalized epilepsy usually do not have any evidence of history, general or neurologic, laboratory or neuroimaging examination abnormalities. The awake EEG performed in individuals with GTCS may be normal, although some interictal EEG patterns maybe typical of specific generalized epilepsy syndromes [16]. Of note, it is sometimes difficult to distinguish ES from PNES, anxiety attacks, and malingering [17].

Status epilepticus is traditionally defined as a continuous, incessant seizure that lasts more than 30 min, or recurrent

seizures without recovering consciousness between the seizures. Nonetheless, the treatment of such medical emergency is usually started after no more than 5 min, since there is growing evidence that after 5 min of hypoxia neurons may be irreversibly damaged [18].

2.3. Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures

Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures are commonly defined as paroxysmal episodes resembling an epileptic seizure, often leading to misdiagnosis.

Nonetheless, PNES are considered to be psychogenic in origin, with a 20%-30% prevalence in patients attending tertiary epilepsy centers. Patients with PNES may present with a higher prevalence of post traumatic experiences with depressive mood and anxiety, fibromyalgia and chronic pain. Their medical and personal history is almost always different from patients suffering from epilepsy: the paroxysmal behaviors are mainly motor bilateral phenomena, including jerking or shaking, without objective signs of loss of consciousness; appear during the day and rarely at nighttime; the frequency does not diminish with the use of antic-onvulsants, but with the use of antidepressants; during the seizure, EEG (if performed) is usually normal.

3. Sleep as a model for altered states of consciousness

Human and primate sleep is subdivided into NREM and REM sleep. During NREM sleep there is a progressive slowing of brain metabolic activity and flow from wakefulness, which is most evident in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPC), responsible for the executive functions. Serotonin and noradrenalin neurotransmission slow down, while hista-minergic and gabaergic modulation take over. Motor activity is disfacilitated occurring only on sleep stage transitions, whilst mental activity becomes perseverative and akin to rumination; perception of external stimuli is dull or absent.

Instead, during REM sleep, cholinergic firing from the latero-dorsal tegmentum (LDT) and the peduncolo-pontine nucleus (PPN), stimulate the occipital cortex inducing vivid visual imagery, which is internally generated owing to complete shut-off of output-input gating. Movement is not only disfacilitated as in NREM sleep, but actually actively inhibited at the level of spinal motor neurons through the sublateral dorsal nucleus (SLD) stimulation. Moreover, REM sleep carries a cumulative excitation level similar to that observed during the waking state, with the exception of the fronto-parietal cortices, which still maintain the low level of activity of slow wave sleep (SWS) [19].

Notably, wake, NREM and REM sleep are not to be seen as discreet all-or-nothing states of being, but rather their intrinsic components may rapidly oscillate and recombine in hybrid states of pathological (sleep disorders) or simply unusual (parasomnias) consciousness misfits [20].

Some clinical examples of dissociated states of consciousness mediated from sleep pathology include narcolepsy, disorders of arousal (DOA) and REM behavior disorders.

3.1. Narcolepsy

It is a rare neurological sleep disorder characterized by a tetrad of symptoms: excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) with irresistible nap urgency, cataplexy, hypnagogic/hypnopompic hallucinations and paralysis [21].

The lack of hypothalamic orexin/hypocretin in this disease is responsible for both sleepiness and incorrect coupling of sleep markers across sleep-wake phases [22]. An exaggerated REM propensity explains the persistence of REM sleep markers at the sleep-wake transition (hypnagogic hallucinations and paralysis) or during wakefulness through the inhibition of muscle tone, prompted by strong emotions, during cataplectic attacks.

Sleep onset REM periods (SOREMPs), both at night and during daytime naps, are the distinctive physiological markers of the disease, even if in specific conditions, such as severe sleep deprivation by drugs, environmental or clinical conditions, SOREMPs may be found less frequently and consistently than in narcolepsy [23,24].

3.2. Disorders of arousal

Disorders of arousal (DOA) are NREM parasomnias, undesirable, unusual phenomena occurring as behavioral, autonomic or experiential events. They occur out of deep stages of NREM, typically during SWS in the first third of the night [25]. They are partially due to an intrinsic predisposition toward arousal instability, tend to recur as a familial trait, precipitated by sleep deprivation alone or due to other sleep disorders responsible for sleep instability [26,27]. DOA include sleep walking, night terrors and confusional arousals, which are altered states of consciousness to be distinguished from other paroxysmal events, including seizures from temporal or frontal nocturnal epilepsy, with which they share local patterns of brain activation at night.

Sleep walking is characterized by complete or abortive walking behavior occurring unexpectedly at night out of SWS, prompted by an incomplete spontaneous arousal. At the end of the episode the patient spontaneously resumes nocturnal sleep in bed or elsewhere and does not recall his behavior in the morning or even immediately when awakened. Functional neuroimaging (SPECT) of the sleep walking episode demonstrates activation of thalamo-cingulate pathways, paralleled by reduced local cerebral blood flow in the fronto-parietal cortices, as if the brain were motorically activated but unconscious due to the switch-off of the executive functions [28]. Exactly the same areas are at stake during paroxysmal arousals of nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy (NFLE) [29]. A similar EEG/brain activity dissociation supports confusional arousals both in children and, more rarely, in adults in the form of sleep inertia or sleep drunkenness [30]. Basic drives may complicate these episodes with the occurrence of sleep-related eating disorder (SRED) or complex auto or hetero sex behaviors (sexsomnia) [31], which are performed by the subject while still partially asleep and in an altered state of consciousness, which is not morally and legally prosecutable [32].

During night terrors there is, instead, an intense autonomic activation including mydriasis, diaphoresis, tachycardia and tachypnea. Screaming, vocalization or inconsolable crying usually alert parents or bed partners. Any attempt to stop or

console the patient is unsuccessful and may actually only prolong the episode, which as a rule, will be forgotten in the morning by the unaware child. Night terrors are very rare in adults (2.7%) and usually represent reemergence of childhood behaviors, whereas sleepwalking has a higher life time (22.4%) but lower adult prevalence (1.7%) [33].

3.3. REM behavior disorder (RBD)

REM behavior disorder (RBD) is an interesting reverse example of motor activation during REM sleep, takingplace instead ofthe physiological state trait of motor inhibition. Therefore dreams, especially violent dreams, may become "enacted" due to the impairment of the physiologic REM related inhibition of muscle tone, thereby causing self or hetero injurious behaviors during sleep [34]. Acute forms of the disease may be iatrogenic due to intoxication or withdrawal from substances/medications such as alcohol, barbiturates or antidepressants.

Alteration of the brainstem motor networks responsible for motor inhibition (SLD, perilocus coeruleus and/or long descending fibers from the forebrain), are the anatomical basis of RBD. Video-polygraphic recordings allow instrumental confirmation of the clinical diagnosis via detection of REM sleep without atonia.

The extreme form of RBD+ condition is status dissociatus (SD), a clinical condition of extreme continuous state dissociation where dream-like and delirious agitated behaviors occur during apparent sleep, albeit no conventional sleep stages may be classified due to complete loss of any kind of physiologic state markers [35].

Sleep itself and sleep disorders may indeed teach us a lot about altered states of consciousness and may be useful to better understand conditions typical of psychiatric and comatose patients.

4. Acute consciousness disorders

4.1. Confusion

The term confusion lacks precision, but in general it denotes an inability to think with customary speed, clarity, and coherence. All states of confusion are marked by some degree of inattentiveness and disorientation, bewilderment, and difficulty following commands. In this condition the patient does not take into account all elements of his/her immediate environment, with a degree of imperceptiveness and distractibility, referred to traditionally as "clouding of the sensorium''. Confusion results most often from a process that influences the brain globally, such as a toxic or metabolic disturbance or a dementia. However, a confusional state can also accompany focal cerebral disease in various locations, particularly in the right hemisphere, or result from disorders that may disturb mainly language, memory, or visuo-spatial orientation, but these are readily distinguished from the global confusional state [36].

4.2. Delirium

In some medical writings, particularly in the psychiatric literature, the terms delirium and confusion are used

interchangeably, the former connoting nothing more than a characterless confusional state in which hyperactivity may be prominent. On the contrary, the vivid hallucinations, the inaccessibility of the patient to events other than those to which he is reacting at the moment, the extreme agitation, the tendency to tremble, startle easily and the convulse, and the signs of over activity of the autonomic nervous system, as well as a normal EEG tracing, suggest the presence of the "syndrome of delirium tremens'' (observed most often but not exclusively in alcoholics) [36]. It is generally claimed that organic lesions of the frontal lobes can easily cause delirium [37].

4.3. Drowsiness and stupor

In these states, mental, speech, and physical activity are reduced. Drowsiness denotes an inability to sustain a wakeful state without the application of external stimuli, and it is characterized by decreased respiratory and heart rate [38] lower body temperature [39] and increased threshold of response to external stimuli [40]. This state is indistinguishable from light sleep, with slow arousal elicited by speaking to the patient or applying a tactile stimulus [41].

Stupor describes a state in which the patient can be roused only by vigorous and repeated stimuli, when he opens his eyes, looks at the examiner, and does not appear to be unconscious; response to spoken commands is either absent or slow and inadequate; restless or stereotyped motor activity is common in stuporous patients and there is a reduction in the natural shifting of positions. When left unstimulated, these patients quickly drift back into a sleep-like state [42].

4.4. Coma

The most common disorder of consciousness that immediately follows severe brain injury is coma, a time-limited state characterized by eyes-closed unresponsiveness, in the absence of a sleep-wake cycle [42,43]. Comatose patients fail to respond to even the most vigorous stimulation, and when given noxious stimulation, patients may not move at all or may display stereotyped/reflexive movements only.

Coma is always a symptomatic expression of an underlying disease, being brain lesions (cortical, brainstem or white matter damage), metabolic or nutritional disorders, poisoning, infections of the CNS, hypo- or hyperthermia and trauma the most common causes [44]. All of the aforementioned causes have the common feature of interfering with the ARAS and its several projections to the cerebral cortex [45] and the state of coma is the result of damage to this system [44].

As a general rule, recovery from coma of metabolic and toxic causes is better than anoxic coma, with head injury occupying an intermediate prognostic position. Most comatose patients following severe stroke have high probability to die, being subarachnoid hemorrhage an exception, as it may induce coma also in absence of hydrocephalus [42]. Post-anoxic coma is an unconsciousness state secondary to global cerebral anoxia, mainly following a cardiac arrest. Post-anoxic coma that lasts more than several hours has normally a bad outcome [46]. Indeed, consciousness recovery arises in over 20%-30% of the patients remaining in coma for at least 24 h. To

this end, research is aimed at investigating the potential elements that may reliably predict bad outcomes in post-anoxic subjects. Indeed, the PROPAC study [46] has found that the bilateral absence of the early cortical response after median nerve somatosensory potentials should be considered one of the most consistent predictor of bad outcome.

Notably, in the late 1950s European neurologists paid attention to a state of coma in which the brain was irreversibly damaged and had ceased to function, but pulmonary and cardiac function could still be maintained by artificial means. Mollaret and Goulon referred to this condition as ''coma depasse'' (namely, brain death) [47], with the diagnosis based on the: (i) absence of cerebral functions; (ii) absence of brainstem functions; (iii) irreversibility of the state.

5. Chronic unconsciousness

Chronic disorders of consciousness are a tragic success of high-technology treatment, in an attempt to maintain or reestablish brain function, which is to be considered as the main goal of therapeutics. Management of vegetative or a minimally conscious state individuals involves charily getting the right diagnosis with an evidence-based prognosis, also taking into account the medical, ethical, and legal key factors of the ideal treatment [48].

The VS and MCS are disorders of consciousness (DOC), which may be acute and reversible, or chronic and irreversible (Table), and are usually due to diffuse lesions of the thalami or the fronto-temporal-parietal cortices and their white-matter interconnecting tracts [49].

5.1. Vegetative state

Vegetative state, recently named unawareness unwakefulness syndrome (UWS) by The European Task Force on Disorders of Consciousness [50], is a behaviorally defined disorder, whereby patients do not have either any evidence of self or environmental awareness or any voluntary motor responsiveness in the presence of eye-open wakefulness; such condition may be either transitory (leading to a MCS) or irreversible. As in

coma state, patients can have spontaneous or stimulus-induced, stereotyped movements, and may retain brainstem regulation of visceral autonomic function that would suggest that the lower brainstem is undamaged [51].

However, the main behaviorally difference from coma is that VS patient present with a nearly normal alternation of eyes-open and eyes closed periods. Nonetheless, this does not imply that VS patients have normal sleep-wake cycles, since their EEGs display a monotonous slow pattern regardless of whether the eyes are open or closed, or they only have fragmented components of normal electrographic sleep-wake phenomenology. Indeed, the eye opening periods reflect only a rudimentary arousal pattern that involves upper brainstem nuclei.

Interestingly, where VS evoked cortical activity can be recorded, it is fragmented and not integrated to the various networks linked to the state of consciousness. Recovery of consciousness may be partly related to a reversal of such silent long-range cortical connections [52,53].

Different studies have investigated the response to auditory and painful somato-sensory stimuli, leading to interesting findings. Indeed, it has been shown that, although the primary auditory cortices are activated by auditory stimuli, higher order multi-sensory association areas are not [54]. In this condition, patients have a functional disconnection between the primary auditory, multi-modal associative and limbic areas. Moreover, the administration of painful stimuli in VS patients has been demonstrated to activate only the primary somato-sensory cortex, with a functional disconnection from higher order associative areas, including the so-called ''pain matrix'' [55,56].

VS may represent a transitional state on the way to recovery of consciousness or could be a chronic condition in cases of more severe brain injuries. Persistent vegetative state is a term used for patients who have remained in VS for an arbitrarily defined duration of 30 days, whereas permanent vegetative state [57] is applied to patients in VS after global ischemia for 3 months or TBI for 1 year. However, such terms are not recommended and they should be replaced by the injury etiology (traumatic versus non-traumatic) and the duration of the condition, since these key elements seem to affect outcome [58].

Table - Differential clinical features of vegetative and minimally conscious state.

Consciousness Sleep/ wake Motor function Auditory function Visual function Communication Emotion

Vegetative None Present Postures or Startle; brief Startle; brief None None; reflexive

state withdraws to noxious stimuli; occasional non-purposeful movement orienting to sound visual fixation crying or smiling

Minimally Traces Present Localizes noxious Localizes Sustained Contingent vocalization; Contingent

conscious stimuli; reaches sound visual fixation; inconsistent but intelligible smiling and

state for objects; holds or touches objects in a manner that accommodates size and shape; automatic movements location; inconsistent command following sustained visual pursuit verbalization or gesture crying

5.2. Minimally conscious state

Minimally conscious state (MCS) the next level of recovery on the continuum from VS to full consciousness, is defined as a state of severely impaired consciousness, whereby the patients present with marginal but explicit behaviors, thus demonstrating some evidence of self or environmental awareness.

MCS includes a more heterogeneous group of patients than VS, because the definition allows for a wide range of behaviors, whereas VS only includes reflexive movements. In MCS, "low-end behaviors'' include visual tracking to a mirror, localization of noxious touch, and inaccurate verbalization, whereas "high-end" behaviors include consistent movement to command and choosing correctly between 2 objects [58]. Patients with only low-end behaviors can be difficult to differentiate from VS, because such behaviors may be slight and infrequent. This differentiation may be important because patients in MCS have significantly better prognoses for recovery than those in VS [59]. The EEG pattern shows in both conditions a diffuse slowing of brain electrical activity, but often with a generalized polymorphic delta or theta rhythm in the VS [60]; however, EEG is not of great help in their differential diagnosis. Based on growing clinical and scientific evidence, a major issue is to evaluate and understand the presence of cortical activation in patients with clinical diagnosis of VS, documented by both event-related potentials (ERPs), and fMRI [61,62]. To this end, some authors [63] have proposed to identify an additional intermediate category between VS and MCS under the name of "non-behavioral MCS'', others more carefully define patients with "islands'' of cognitive activity [64]. These patients cannot exhibit any behavioral evidence of cognition due to severe motor impairment, although they are minimally conscious.

The distinction between coma, brain death, VS and MCS (although critical) is moderately robust in clinical practice, but it is not immune from practical and theoretical mistakes. On a practical level, there are evident examples that VS is often misdiagnosed in patients who are indeed aware of themselves and of their environment [65,66].

MCS has to be cautiously examined to be distinguished from those with locked-in syndrome (LIS), since both the conditions are characterized by minimal behavioral interaction with the environment [67]. In particular, LIS is a disorder where the patient is aware and awake, although is not able to move or verbally communicate, owing to a nearly complete paralysis of the all voluntary muscles, with the exception of the eyes. Locked-in patients have higher levels of metabolic activity in some regions of the medial parietal cortex than VS or MCS patients [53]. In addition, there is no evidence of cortical function reduction in such patients, suggesting that LIS is a condition of motor deafferentation in which consciousness and intellectual capacity are perfectly preserved [44]. Notably, emerging neuroimaging and neurophysiological data are demonstrating a new diagnostic category, namely "functional locked-in syndrome'', to emphasize the dissociation between the extreme behavioral motor dysfunctions and the partially preserved cognitive functions [56,68-74].

To date, the best-standardized and most frequently used tool for the clinical assessment of chronic disorders of consciousness, mostly to distinguish VS from MSC, is represented by the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) [75].

The scale was first described in 1991 [76] and revised in 2004 [75], and it consists of six sub-scale addressing auditory, visual, motor, oro-motor/verbal, communication and arousal processes. The lowest score on each sub-scale describes reflexive activity, while the highest score represents purposeful behaviors. Moreover, according to the different scores obtained, MCS can be divided into: MCS- describing "low-level behavioral'' responses (i.e. visual pursuit, localization of noxious stimulation or feasible behavior), and MCS+ with "high-level behavioral responses'' (including, command following, comprehensible verbalizations and non-functional communication) [68].

6. Conclusions

Disordered states of consciousness define a broad category encompassing a spectrum of cognitive dysfunction, whose differential diagnosis is still challenging, especially concerning the chronic state. To date, bedside clinical examination represents the gold standard for establishing diagnosis of DOC. Although neuroimaging and electrophysiology techniques play a major role in the identification of the sites of lesional damage, they have to be considered as auxiliary in the diagnostic process. Based on cognitive and motor skills of the patient, it is possible to show up a continuum from coma state to full recovery of consciousness. The transition from coma to VS is discernible by the opening of the eyes; the conversion from VS to the MCS is characterized by voluntary intentional behavior, whilst the step from MCS to severe disability by the presence of purposeful communication. However, the evaluation of these patients is extremely difficult, and depends on subjective interpretations of the patient's behavior, either spontaneously or in response to any stimuli. This difficulty unfortunately may lead to frequent diagnostic errors and confusion, with high rates of misdiagnosis [77]. For this reason, a profound knowledge of the clinical manifestation of DOC conditions, a wider use of standardized behavioral scales, besides the development of new diagnostic tools, should be fostered in the future in order to reduce diagnostic errors.

Conflict of interest

The authors state no conflict of interests.

REFERENCES

[1] Zeman A. Consciousness: concepts, neurobiology, terminology of impairments theoretical models and philosophical background. Handb Clin Neurol 2008;90:3-31.

[2] Neylan TC. Physiology of arousal: Moruzzi and Magoun's ascending reticular activating system. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;7:250.

[3] Laureys S. The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9:556-9.

[4] Zeman A. What do we mean by "conscious" and "aware". Neuropsychol Rehabil 2006;16:356-76.

[5] Laureys S, Perrin F, Bredart S. Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious Cogn 2007;16:722-4.

[6] Westby M, Davis S, Bullock I, Miller P, Cooper P, Turnbull N, [28 et al. Transient loss of consciousness ('blackouts') management in adults and young people. London: National [29 Clinical Guideline Centre for Acute and Chronic Conditions, Royal College of Physicians; 2010.

[7] Dunn MJ, Breen DP, Davenport RJ, Gray AJ. Early [30 management of adults with an uncomplicated first generalised seizure. Emerg Med J 2005;22:237-42.

[8] Van Donselaar CA, Geerts AT, Meulstee J, HabbemaJD, Staal A. Reliability of the diagnosis of first seizure. Neurology [31 1989;39:267-71.

[9] Theodore WH, Porter RJ, Albert P, Kelley K, Bromfield E, Devinsky O, et al. The secondarily generalized tonic-clonic seizure: a videotape analysis. Neurology 1994;44:1403-7. [32

[10] Hoefnagels WA, Padberg GW, Overweg J, van der Velde EA, Roos RA. Transient loss of consciousness: the value of the [33 history for distinguishing seizure from syncope. J Neurol 1991;238:39-43.

[11] Lempert T, Bauer M, Schmidt D. Syncope: a videometric [34 analysis of 56 episodes of transient cerebral hypoxia. Ann Neurol 1994;36:233-7.

[12] Margineanu DG. Epileptic hypersynchrony revisited. Neuroreport 2010;21:963-7. [35

[13] Hogan RE, English EA. Epilepsy and brain function: common ideas of Hughlings-Jackson and Wilder Penfield. Epilepsy [36 Behav 2012;24:311-3.

[14] Hauser WA, Annegers JF, Kurland LT. Prevalence of [37 epilepsy in Rochester, Minnesota: 1940-1980. Epilepsia 1991;32:429-45. [38

[15] Blumenfeld H. Impaired consciousness in epilepsy. Lancet Neurol 2012;11:814-26.

[16] Berg AT, Cross JH. Classification of epilepsies and seizures: [39 historical perspective and future directions. Handb Clin Neurol 2012;107:99-111.

[17] van Dijk N, Sprangers MA, Colman N, Boer KR, Wieling W, Linzer M. Clinical factors associated with quality of life in [40 patients with transient loss of consciousness. J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol 2006;17:998-1003.

[18] McLauchlan DJ, Robertson NP. Management of status epilepticus. J Neurol 2012;259:2261-3. [41

[19] Hobson JA. REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10: 803-13. [42

[20] Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Insights from studying human sleep disorders. Nature 2005;437:1279-85. [43

[21] American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The international classification of sleep disorders, revised: diagnostic and coding manual. Rochester: American Academy of Sleep [44 Medicine; 2005.

[22] Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, Lammers G, Mignot E. Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency in human narcolepsy. Lancet [45 2000;355:39-40.

[23] Heier MS, Evsiukova T, Vilming S, Gjerstad MD, Schrader H, Gautvik K. CSF hypocretin-1 levels and clinical profiles in [46 narcolepsy and idiopathic CNS hypersomnia in Norway. Sleep 2007;30:969-73.

[24] Mignot E, Lin L, Finn L, Lopes C, Pluff K, Sundstrom ML, et al. Correlates of sleep-onset REM periods during the Multiple [47 Sleep Latency Test in community adults. Brain 2006;129:1609-23. [48

[25] Broughton RJ. Sleep disorders: disorders of arousal? Enuresis, somnambulism, and nightmares occur in [49 confusional states of arousal, not in dreaming sleep.

Science 1968;159:1070-8.

[26] Broughton RJ. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Philadelphia: Saunders Company; 2000. [50]

[27] Pressman MR. Factors that predispose, prime and precipitate NREM parasomnias in adults: clinical and forensic implications. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:5-30.

Bassetti C, Vella S, Donati F, Wiellep P, Weder B. SPECT during sleep walking. Lancet 2000;356:484-5. Vetrugno R, Mascalchi M, Vella A, Della Nave R, Provini F, Plazzi G, et al. Paroxysmal arousal in epilepsy associated with cingulate hyperperfusion. Neurology 2005;64:356-8. Stores G. Confusional arousals. In: Thorpy MJ, Plazzi G, editors. The parasomnias and other sleep-related movement disorders. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2010. p. 99-108.

Schenck CH, Arnulf I, Mahowald MW. Sleep and sex: what can go wrong? A review of the literature on sleep related disorders and abnormal sexual behaviors and experiences. Sleep 2007;30:683-702.

Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Parasomnias: sleep walking and the law. Sleep Med Rev 2000;4:321-39. Bjorvatn B, Gronli J, Pallesen S. Prevalence of different parasomnias in the general population. Sleep Med 2010;11:1031-4.

Boeve B, Silber M, Parisi J, Dickson DW, Ferman TJ, Benarroch EE, et al. Synucleinopathy pathology and REM sleep behavior disorder plus dementia or parkinsonism. Neurology 2003;61:40-5.

Mahowald MW, Schenck CH. Status dissociatus a perspective on states of being. Sleep 1991;14:69-79. Tindall SC. Clinical methods: the history, physical, and laboratory examinations. Boston: Butterworths; 1990. Attard A, Ranjith G, Taylor D. Delirium and its treatment. CNS Drugs 2008;22:631-44.

Ogilvie RD, Wilkinson RT, Allison S. The detection of sleep-onset: behavioural, physiological and subjective convergence. Sleep 1989;12:458-74.

Van Den Heuvel CJ, Noone JT, Lushington K, Dowson D. Changes in sleepiness and body temperature precede nocturnal sleep onset: evidence from a polysomnographic study in young men. J Sleep Res 1998;7:159-66. Ogilvie RD, Simons IA, Kuderian RH, MacDonald T, Rustember J. Behavioural, event-related potential, and EEG/FFT changes at sleep onset. Psychophysiology 1991;28: 54-64.

De Gennaro L, Devoto A, Lucidi F, Violani C. Oculomotor changes are associated to daytime sleepiness in the multiple sleep latency test. J Sleep Res 2005;14:107-12. Posner JB, Saper CB, Schiff ND, Plum F. The diagnosis of stupor and coma. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2007. Schiff ND, Plum F. The role of arousal and gating systems in the neurology of impaired consciousness. J Clin Neurophysiol 2000;17:438-52.

Laureys S, Tononi G. The neurology of consciousness: cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology. Oxford: Academic Press; 2008.

Vincent SR. The ascending reticular activating system: from aminergic neurons to nitric oxide. J Chem Neuroanat 2000;18:23-30.

Zandbergen EG, Koelman JH, de Haan RJ, Hijdra A, PROPAC-Study Group. SSEPs and prognosis in postanoxic coma: only short or also long latency responses. Neurology 2006;67:583-6.

Mollaret P, Goulon M. Le coma depasse. Rev Neurol 1959;101:3-15.

Bernat JL. Chronic disorders of consciousness. Lancet 2006;367:1181-92.

Calabrö RS, Cacciola A, Bramanti P, Milardi D. Neural correlates of consciousness: what we know and what we have to learn! Neurol Sci 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ s10072-015-2072-x

Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, Leön-Carriön J, Sannita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med 2010;8:68.

[51] Sara M, Pistoia F. Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state. Lancet 2012;379:1702-3.

[52] Laureys S, Lemaire C, Maquet P, Phillips C, Franck G. Cerebral metabolism during vegetative state and after recovery to consciousness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:121.

[53] Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol 2004;3:537-46.

[54] Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Degueldre C, Fiore GD, Damas P, Lambermont B, et al. Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain 2000;123:1589-601.

[55] Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Peigneux P, Damas P, Lambermont B, Del Fiore G, et al. Cortical processing of noxious somatosensory stimuli in the persistent vegetative state. Neuroimage 2002;17:732-41.

[56] Naro A, Leo A, Russo M, Quartarone A, Bramanti P, Calabro RS. Shaping thalamo-cortical plasticity: a marker of cortical pain integration in patients with post-anoxic unresponsive wakefulness syndrome? Brain Stimul 2015;8:97-104.

[57] Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (2). The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. N Engl J Med 1994;330:1572-9.

[58] Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, et al. The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology 2002;58:349-53.

[59] Kotchoubey B, Merz S, Lang S, Markl A, Müller F, Yu T, et al. Global functional connectivity reveals highly significant differences between the vegetative and the minimally conscious state. J Neurol 2013;260:975-83.

[60] Demertzi A, Laureys S, Boly M. Coma persistent vegetative states and diminished consciousness. In: Banks WP, editor. Encyclopedia of consciousness. Oxford: Elsevier; 2009. p. 147-56.

[61] Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard JD. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science 2006;313:1402.

[62] Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, Tshibanda L, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N Engl J Med 2010;362:579-89.

[63] Schiff ND. Bringing neuroimaging tools closer to diagnostic use in the severely injured brain. Brain 2007;130:2482-3.

[64] Fischer C, Luaute J, Morlet D. Event-related potentials (MMN and novelty P3) in permanent vegetative or minimally conscious states. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:1032-42.

[65] Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C. Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ 1996;313:13-6.

[66] Childs NL, Mercer WN, Childs HW. Accuracy of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Neurology 1993;43:1465-7.

[67] Laureys S, Pellas F, Van Eeckhout P, Ghorbel S, Schnakers C, Perrin F, et al. The locked-in syndrome: what is it like to be conscious but paralyzed and voiceless. Prog Brain Res 2005;150:495-511.

[68] Rodriguez Moreno D, Schiff ND, Giacino J, Kalmar K, Hirsch J. A network approach to assessing cognition in disorders of consciousness. Neurology 2010;75:1871-8.

[69] Bruno MA, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Thibaut A, Moonen G, Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. J Neurol 2011;258:1373-84.

[70] Naro A, Russo M, Leo A, Bramanti P, Quartarone A, Calabro RS. A single session of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: preliminary results. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1545968314562114

[71] Naro A, Calabro RS, Russo M, Leo A, Pollicino P, Quartarone A, et al. Can transcranial direct current stimulation be useful in differentiating unresponsive wakefulness syndrome from minimally conscious state patients? Restor Neurol Neurosci 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/RNN-140448

[72] Formisano R, D'Ippolito M, Risetti M, Riccio A, Caravasso CF, Catani S, et al. Vegetative state, minimally conscious state, akinetic mutism and Parkinsonism as a continuum of recovery from disorders of consciousness: an exploratory and preliminary study. Funct Neurol 2011;26:15-24.

[73] Formisano R, D'Ippolito M, Catani S. Functional locked-in syndrome as recovery phase of vegetative state. Brain Inj 2013;27:1332.

[74] Formisano R, Pistoia F, Sara M. Disorders of consciousness: a taxonomy to be changed. Brain Inj 2011;25:638-9.

[75] Giacino J, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004;85:2020-9.

[76] Giacino JT, Kezmarsky MA, DeLuca J, Cicerone KD. Monitoring rate of recovery to predict outcome in minimally responsive patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1991;72:897-901.

[77] Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol 2009;9:35.