Scholarly article on topic '“Expectant Parents”: Study protocol of a longitudinal study concerning prenatal (risk) factors and postnatal infant development, parenting, and parent-infant relationships'

“Expectant Parents”: Study protocol of a longitudinal study concerning prenatal (risk) factors and postnatal infant development, parenting, and parent-infant relationships Academic research paper on "Psychology"

CC BY
0
0
Share paper
Academic journal
BMC Pregnancy & Childbirth
OECD Field of science
Keywords
{""}

Academic research paper on topic "“Expectant Parents”: Study protocol of a longitudinal study concerning prenatal (risk) factors and postnatal infant development, parenting, and parent-infant relationships"

Pregnancy & Childbirth

STUDY PROTOCOL Open Access

"Expectant Parents": Study protocol of a longitudinal study concerning prenatal (risk) factors and postnatal infant development, parenting, and parent-infant relationships

A Janneke BM Maas1+, Charlotte MJM Vreeswijk1+, Evi SA de Cock1, Catharina HAM Rijk and Hedwig JA van Bakel1,2*

Abstract

Background: While the importance of the infant-parent relationship from the child's perspective is acknowledged worldwide, there is still a lack of knowledge about predictors and long-term benefits or consequences of the quality of parent-infant relationships from the parent's perspective. The purpose of this prospective study is to investigate the quality of parent-infant relationships from parents' perspectives, both in the prenatal and postpartum period. This study therefore focuses on prenatal (risk) factors that may influence the quality of pre- and postnatal bonding, the transition to parenthood, and bonding as a process within families with young children. In contrast to most research concerning pregnancy and infant development, not only the roles and experiences of mothers during pregnancy and the first two years of infants' lives are studied, but also those of fathers.

Methods/design: The present study is a prospective longitudinal cohort study, in which pregnant women (N =466) and their partners (N = 319) are followed from 15 weeks gestation until their child is 24 months old. During pregnancy, midwives register the presence of prenatal risk factors and provide obstetric information after the child's birth. Parental characteristics are investigated using self-report questionnaires at 15, 26, and 36 weeks gestational age and at 4, 6, 12, and 24 months postpartum. At 26 weeks of pregnancy and at 6 months postpartum, parents are interviewed concerning their representations of the (unborn) child. At 6 months postpartum, the mother-child interaction is observed in several situations within the home setting. When children are 4, 6, 12, and 24 months old, parents also completed questionnaires concerning the child's (social-emotional) development and the parent-child relationship. Additionally, at 12 months information about the child's physical development and well-being during the first year of life is retrieved from National Health Care Centres. Discussion: The results of this study may contribute to early identification of families at risk for adverse parent-infant relationships, infant development, or parenting. Thereby this study will be relevant for the development of policy, practice, and theory concerning infant mental health.

Keywords: Attachment, Bonding, Mother, Father, Pregnancy, Risk factors, Representations, Infant

* Correspondence: h.j.a.vanbakel@tilburguniversity.edu +Equalcontributors

department of DevelopmentalPsychology, Tilburg University, Room P-704,

P.O. Box 90153, 5000LE, Tilburg, the Netherlands

2Centre of Infant MentalHealth, Dimence, Deventer, the Netherlands

O© 2012 Maas et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Central Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, anc reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Background

Developmental research has firmly established the quality of the relationship between an infant and his or her parent as an important factor influencing the child's later development [1-6]. When children develop a secure relationship with their parents or caregivers in their first years of life, they generally have better cognitive outcomes, better social interactions, display less behavioral problems, and achieve better at school [7]. Research in this area has mainly investigated the attachment relationships that infants form with their parents, thus focusing on the child's perspective of the relationship. In contrast, the attachment relationship from the parent's perspective has not been frequently studied. This concept, also known as bonding, may be of equal importance to later child development as the traditionally studied concept of infant-to-parent attachment. More research concerning predictors and long-term benefits or consequences of bonding is therefore needed [8].

The development of the parent-infant attachment relationship does not start after the child is born, but already evolves during pregnancy [9,10]. The relationship a parent forms with the fetus is often referred to as prenatal attachment and has been described as the earliest, most basic form of human intimacy [11]. Several definitions of prenatal attachment have been provided, many conceptualized in health research, but it is generally defined as the emotional tie or bond that develops between expectant parents and their fetus [12,13]. Researchers have pointed out that it is important to study prenatal attachment and factors related to its development, since it provides insightful information on later parent-infant bonding [11]. Several studies found that the quality of the parent-fetus relationship was related to the quality of postnatal parent-infant relationships [14-17]. It is assumed that the prenatal parent-infant relationship influences the parent's daily interactions with the child after birth and subsequently affects the quality of the parent-infant relationship and development.

Next to these feelings of attachment during pregnancy, research concerning the parent-fetus relationship has focused on another concept known as internal working models or representations of the unborn child [18,19]. Representations are described as a set of tendencies to behave in particular ways in intimate relationships [20]. They provide information about the 'meaning' a child has to his or her parent by asking the parent about his or her experiences with and perceptions of the fetus, (future) parenting, and the relationship with the fetus. The majority of research concerning internal working models has focused on postnatal representations, while studies on parents' prenatal representations are scarce. Since prenatal representations are found to be related to postnatal representations and postnatal parent-infant

interaction, it is important that the quality of prenatal representations and its consequences are also further investigated [14,17,21]. In addition, it is unknown whether discrepancies between pre- and postnatal representations lead to parental adjustment problems once the child is born, possibly affecting the quality of postnatal bonding and later child outcomes.

Parent-infant attachment or bonding develops further after birth and continues to develop beyond the early postnatal period [22]. Surprisingly, empirical research into the determinants, consequences, and stability of postnatal bonding is also limited [23]. Only a few studies have examined predictors and consequences of postnatal bonding and they suggest that prematurity, domestic violence during pregnancy, and maternal postpartum mood are related to adverse maternal bonding and adverse parent-infant interactions [24-27]. Moreover, Brockington [28] stressed that both severe disturbances, as well as less severe problems with parental bonding may lead to more negative parental care and may subsequently result in various forms of child abuse or neglect. Therefore, several parental, infant, and contextual risk factors are expected to influence the quality of the bonding process.

The present study has been designed to investigate prenatal (risk) factors that may influence the quality of pre- and postnatal parent-infant relationships and postnatal infant development within families with young children. Several determinants and consequences of the early parent-infant relationship will be investigated. Already during pregnancy, prenatal risk factors influencing the quality of the parent-infant relationship and later child development can be identified [29]. For example, emotional problems of mothers during pregnancy, problems in mothers' own childhood history, and deficits in parental cognitive functioning increase the possibility of problematic caregiving and child development [30,31]. However, there is still considerable debate and a lack of knowledge about how specific risk factors are related to the long-term benefits or consequences of the parent-infant relationship. In contrast to most research concerning pregnancy and infant development, this study does not only focus on maternal characteristics, but also on the roles and experiences of fathers during pregnancy and the first two years of the infants' lives.

The following topics and research questions related to the parent-infant relationship will therefore be investigated in the current study:

1) The relationship between prenatal (risk) factors, postnatal infant development and quality of the parent-infant relationship. Can specific prenatal risk factors for adverse infant development, parenting, or parent-infant relationships be identified during pregnancy?

2) The transition to parenthood. Is there a discrepancy between the quality of prenatal and postnatal parent-infant relationships and parents' representations of the child? Do parents' prenatal expectations of the child's characteristics meet their postnatal experiences? How are these factors related to infant behavior and development?

3) Parental bonding over time. What are the stability and change in parents' feelings of bonding over time? Is the quality of early parent-infant bonding related to later child development?

Methods/design

Enrollment and informed consent

Between November 2008 and July 2009, 835 pregnant women were invited by their midwives to participate in this study. Four midwifery practices in Eindhoven, the 5th largest city of the Netherlands, agreed to participate in the study. At the first routine visit (between 9-15 weeks gestational age), midwives gave mothers information about the purpose of the study and invited them to participate. The oral information was accompanied by an information brochure with specific information about the study, which each mother received. If mothers were interested in participation, one of the researchers contacted them by phone to provide additional information and asked whether mothers wanted to enroll in the study. Partners were not directly approached by the researchers but the mothers were informed about the importance of involvement of their partners in the study. After parents received oral and written information about the protocol, both parents were asked for written consent. The informed consent form consisted of three different options. Parents could consent to (1) active participation in the complete research protocol, including two home visits, (2) active participation by filling in questionnaires but not by participating in home visits, or (3) passive

participation by allowing the researchers to gather information from the midwife and National Health Care Centres, but without home visits or filling in questionnaires. Separate informed consent forms were sent to mothers and their partners. Once parents returned the signed forms, enrollment in the study was complete.

The "Expectant Parents" ["In Verwachting"] study protocol has been financed and approved by the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMW, Grant 80-82405-98-074/157001020). It was also approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of St. Elisabeth Hospital Tilburg (date: 13-08-2008, register number: NL 23376.008.08).

Participants

Of the 835 invited women, women with a poor understanding of the Dutch and English language, those expecting multiple births, and women who were over 20 weeks of gestation at enrollment, were excluded from participation. Reasons for not giving written consent were withdrawal by the mother, miscarriage, and non-responding mothers. As Figure 1 demonstrates, this resulted in 466 completed informed consent forms of expectant mothers and 319 informed consents of their partners. All parents hereby gave permission to the researchers to retrieve information about the pregnancy and delivery from their midwives and information about the development of the child in the first year of life from National Health Care Centres. Of these parents, 409 mothers and 319 fathers agreed to complete questionnaires, of which 311 mothers and 243 fathers also agreed to participate during home visits (full participation).

Study design

The present study is a prospective longitudinal cohort study, in which pregnant women and their partners were followed from 15 weeks gestation until their child was 24 months old. As can be seen in Figure 2, pregnant

Figure 1 Flow chart of study population 'Expectant Parents'.

women completed questionnaires at 15, 26, and 36 weeks gestational age. At 26 weeks of pregnancy, their partners also completed a questionnaire. At the same time a home visit took place during which a standardized interview concerning the prenatal representations of the unborn child was conducted with both parents separately.

Postnatally, there were five more measurement waves (at birth and at 4, 6, 12, and 24 months postpartum). Obstetric information about the birth of the child, including birth weight, Apgar score, and possible complications was registered by the midwives in line with their general practice guidelines. Additionally, midwives provided information about the presence of possible prenatal risk factors within families by completing an adapted Dutch version of the Dunedin Family Services Indicator (DFSI) [31]. At 4 and 6 months postnatally, both parents received questionnaires. At the child's age of 6 months, an interview about the representations of their child was administered with both parents at their home, and the mother-child interaction was observed in several contexts within the home. Interviews generally lasted between 30 and 60 minutes and the observation of mother-infant interactions lasted approximately 20 minutes. All home visits were video-recorded. When children were 12 months old, mothers completed questionnaires concerning the child's (social-emotional) development and information about the child's physical development and well-being during the first year of life was retrieved from National Health Care Centres. At the child's age of 24 months, the last measures concerning parental characteristics, the parent-child relationship, and the child's development were completed by both parents.

Study measures

Figure 3 shows which variables were investigated at different time points during the study. Generally, the study measures can be classified according to whether they concern parental characteristics, infant characteristics, or the parent-infant relationship. Therefore, the selected instruments are described below according to these categories.

Parental characteristics

Parental characteristics were investigated using self-report questionnaires. To assess parental psychological well-being, the following questionnaires were used: Edinburgh Depression Scale (EDS) [32], State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [33], Symptom Check List; anxiety, depression, and hostility subscale (SCL-90) [34], Symptoms of Anxiety-Depression index (SAD-4) [35], and Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [36].

To assess parents' personality characteristics, the Quick Big Five (QBF) [37], Type D Scale (DS14) [38], and Ego Resiliency 89 Scale (ER89) [39] were

Mothers Fathers

T1: 15 weeks gestation • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Personality (DS14) • Demographic information

T2: 26 weeks gestation • Prenatal representations of unborn child (WMCI, PRAM) • Prenatal expectations of infant temperament (ICQ) • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Personality (QBF) • Perceived stress (PSS) • Maternal orientation (PPQ) • Attachment style (RQ) • Prenatal attachment (MAAS) T2: 26 weeks gestation • Prenatal representations of unborn child (WMCI, PRAM) • Prenatal expectations of infant temperament (ICQ) • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Personality (QBF) • Perceived stress (PSS) • Attachment style (RQ) • Prenatal attachment (PAAS) • Personality (DS14) • Demographic information

T3: 36 weeks gestation • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Relationship with partner (VGP) • Resiliency (ER89)

T4: Birth • DFSI • Obstetric information

T5: 4 months postpartum • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Personality (QBF, DS14) • Perceived stress (PSS) • Parent child relationship (PBQ) • Child development (ASq) T5: 4 months postpartum • Depressive symptoms (EDS + SCL-90) • Anxiety (STAI + SCL-90) • Personality (QBF, DS14) • Perceived stress (PSS) • Parent-child relationship (PBQ) • Resiliency (ER89)

T6: 6 months postpartum • Representations of the child (WMCI, PRAM) • Mother-child interaction (NICHD) • Child temperament (ICQ) • Relationship with partner (VGP) • Mother-infant attachment (MPAS) • Parenting self-efficacy (PMP-SE) • Maternal orientation (FRQ) • Child development (ASQ) • Child social-emotional development (ASQ-SE) T6: 6 months postpartum • Representations of the child (WMCI, PRAM) • Child temperament (ICQ) • Relationship with partner (VGP) • Father-infant attachment (PPAS) • Parenting self-efficacy (PMP-SE)

T7: 12 months postpartum • Depressive symptoms (EDS) • Anxiety (STAI) • Child development (ASQ) • Child social-emotional development (ASQ-SE) • Infant behavior (ITSEA, IBQ-R) • Information from National Health Care Centres

T8: 24 months postpartum • Representations of the child (PRAM) • Mother-infant attachment (MPAS) • Parenting (NOSIK) • Relationship with partner (VGP) • Symptoms of anxiety and depression (SAD-4) • Personality (QBF, DS14) • Attachment difficulties (ADSI) • Infant behavior (ECBQ, BITSEA) • Child cognitive functioning (BRIEF-P) • Demographic information T8: 24 months postpartum • Representations of the child (PRAM) • Father-infant attachment (PPAS) • Parenting (NOSIK) • Relationship with partner (VGP) • Symptoms of anxiety and depression (SAD-4) • Personality (QBF, DS14) • Attachment difficulties (ADSI) • Infant behavior (ECBQ) • Demographic information

Figure 3 Study protocol and assessments at different time points during the study.

administered. Adult attachment style was measured with the Relationship Questionnaire Clinical Version (RQ-CV) [40] and the partner-relationship was evaluated with a subscale of the Questionnaire on Family Problems (Vragenlijst voor Gezinsproblemen; VGP) [41].

The Placenta Paradigm Questionnaire (PPQ) [10] and the Facilitator Regulator Questionnaire (FRQ) [42] were used to determine maternal orientations on pregnancy and their infants. Midwives used an adapted version of the Dunedin Family Services Indicator (DFSI) [31] to

register the presence of possible prenatal risk factors among parents.

Infant characteristics

The following questionnaires were used to investigate infant development and behavior: Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) [43], Ages and Stages Questionnaire; Social-Emotional (ASQ-SE) [44], Infant Characteristics Questionnaire (ICQ) [45], Infant Toddler Social-Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) [46], Brief Infant Toddler Social-Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) [47], subscales of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire Revised (IBQ-R) [48], Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ) [49], and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Preschool version (BRIEF-P) [50].

Information about the child's physical development and well-being during the first year of life was retrieved from National Health Care Centres.

Parent-infant relationship

To determine parents' representations of their (unborn) infant, the Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI) [51] was conducted during home visits. At the same time and also at 24 months, the Pictorial Representations of Attachment Measure (PRAM) [52], a nonverbal measure of the parent-infant relationship, was administered. To evaluate the quality of mother-infant interactions, the NICHD scales [53] were used.

In addition, the following questionnaires were used to give insight into the parent-fetus and parent-child relationship: Maternal Antenatal Attachment Scale (MAAS) [12], Maternal Postnatal Attachment Scale (MPAS) [54], Paternal Antenatal Attachment Scale (PAAS) [12], Paternal Postnatal Attachment Scale

(PPAS) [55], Parental Bonding Questionnaire (PBQ) [56], and Attachment Difficulties Screening Instrument (ADSI) [57].

To evaluate parenting behavior, the following scales were used: the Parental Stress Index (Nijmeegse Ouder-lijke Stress Index-verkort; NOSI-K) [58], and Perceived Maternal Parenting Self Efficacy (PMP-SE) [59].

Data collection and management

The logistics of this study were carried out by three researchers (AM, CV, EdC) in close collaboration with the midwives participating in this study. Before starting data collection, a protocol was set up and discussed with participating midwives to ensure that a uniform protocol was followed by all midwifery practices. Participating midwives were instructed on how to recruit pregnant women for participation in the study and how to register the presence of possible prenatal risk factors.

Questionnaires were sent to parents one or two weeks before the time point they should be completed or before the home-visits. All questionnaires were available in Dutch and English. Reminders were sent when parents failed to return the questionnaires. Table 1 shows the number of parents that participated at each measurement wave.

The researchers (AM, CV, CR) and several research assistants were trained to administer and code the WMCI, concerning parents' representations of their (unborn) children and to code observations of mother-child interactions. All interviews and mother-infant interactions were video-recorded and coded afterwards. A random subgroup of the interviews and observations was coded by more than one coder to determine interrater reliability.

Table 1 Number of participants per time point of the study protocol

Time Measure Mothers Fathers

T1: 15 weeks gestation Questionnaires 406 -

T2: 26 weeks gestation Questionnaires 375 299

Home visit 311 243

T3: 36 weeks gestation Questionnaires 351 -

T4: Birth Information concerning the birth 455 -

DFSIcompleted by midwife 445 -

T5: 4 months postpartum Questionnaires 354 274

T6: 6 months postpartum Questionnaires 341 268

Home visit 295 225

T7: 12 months postpartum Questionnaires 299 -

NationalHealth Care Centres a -

T8: 24 months postpartum Questionnaires 248b 186b

a Data currently not complete.

b For T8, 285 mothers and 246 fathers were approached.

Data preparation

Collected data were entered into an electronic database. Random samples of all manually processed questionnaires were double checked by the researchers to monitor the quality of the manual data entry. All measurements were checked by examination of the data, including their ranges, distributions, means, standard deviations, outliers, and logical errors.

Privacy protection

Databases needed for answering specific research questions were centrally built from databases concerning different time points of the study. All information enabling identification of participants was erased from these databases, except identification numbers of each participant. Video-recordings of participants were stored on the computers of the researchers, which are only accessible with a password and not on web-based directories.

Statistical analyses/power calculation

To answer the various research questions we will use structural equation modeling, regression analyses (HMR analyses), logistic regression analyses, and odds-ratio's. Mediation and moderation analyses will follow Baron and Kenny's requirements [60]. The power calculation is based on one of the main questions that will be addressed about the effects of prenatal (risk) factors on infant development. Assuming a moderate effect size of .30 or .40, a power of .80 (i.e., the minimal power for a similar study by Cohen [61]), an alpha of .05, and 11 parameters/predictors, we need a sample size of 220 participants (the power will be .83 with p = 11, r2 = .09 or the power will be .99 with p = 11, r2 = .16). Abovementioned power calculations are exact calculations, based on results of Gatsonis and Sampson [62]. Allowing for loss to follow-up by 24 months postpartum, we estimated that a sample of at least 240-260 women would be sufficient to test our hypotheses.

Discussion

With this study we aim to gain more insight into the relationships between prenatal (risk) factors, postnatal infant development and the quality of the pre- and postnatal parent-infant relationship. This investigation will lead to more knowledge about the transition to parenthood for both mothers and fathers, and the stability and change in parents' feelings of bonding over time. The longitudinal design with a multi-informant, multimethod approach offers the possibility to predict infant developmental outcomes in the first years of life from pregnancy onwards.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

The study protocolwas developed by HvB, in collaboration with AM, CV, EdC, and CR at the department of DevelopmentalPsychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands. AM and CV were appointed as PhD-students in 2008 and executed the study untilchildren were 12 months old. These authors contributed equally to this work. EdC was appointed to the project as a PhD-student in a later phase and executed the study until children were 24 months old. Allcollaborators are considered as co-authors as they have significantly contributed to developing this research, obtaining the data, and writing the manuscript. Allauthors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors' information

AM, CV, EdC, CR: Department of DevelopmentalPsychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands. HvB: Department of Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands and Centre of Infant MentalHealth, Dimence, Deventer, the Netherlands.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank allfamilies participating in this study. This study is conducted in collaboration with four midwifery practices in Eindhoven, the Netherlands, and we gratefully acknowledge their contributions to the study and their help in recruiting participants. We also collaborated with ZuidZorg NationalHealth Care Centres in Eindhoven, the Netherlands, who provided information about children's development throughout the first year of life. The "Expectant Parents" study was made possible by the financialsupport from the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMW, Grant 80-82405-98-074/157001020).

Received: 28 March 2012 Accepted: 23 May 2012 Published: 11 June 2012

References

1. DeKleyn M, Greenberg MT: In Attachment and psychopathology in childhood, Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd edition. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York: Guilford Press; 2008:637-665.

2. Lyons-Ruth K, Jacobvitz D: In Attachment disorganization: Genetic factors, parenting contexts, and developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood, Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd edition. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York: Guilford Press; 2008:666-697.

3. Rees CA: Thinking about children's attachments. Arch Dis Child 2005, 90(10):1058-1065.

4. Sroufe LA: Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal

study from birth to adulthood. Attachment Hum Dev 2005, 7(4):349-367.

5. Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson EA, Collins WA: The development of the person: The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation From Birth to Adulthood. New York: Guilford Press; 2005.

6. Weinfield NS, Sroufe A, Egeland B, Carlson E: In Individual differences in infant- caregiver attachment: Conceptual and empirical aspects of security, Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd edition. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York: Guilford Press;

2008:78-101.

7. Thompson RA: In Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers, Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd edition. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York: Guilford Press; 2008:348-365.

8. Barlow J, Svanberg PO: In Keeping the baby in mind: Infant mental health in practice, Keeping the baby in mind. Edited by Barlow J, Svanberg PO. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2009:1 -14.

9. Brandon A, Pitts S, Denton W, Stringer A, Evans H: A History of the Theory of Prenatal Attachment. J Prenat Perinat Psychol Health 2009,

23(4):201 —222.

10. Raphael-Leff J: Psychological Processes of Childbearing. London: The Anna Freud Centre; 2005.

11. Condon JT, Corkindale C: The correlates of antenatal attachment in pregnant women. Br J Med Psychol 1997, 70(4):359-372.

12. Condon JT: The assessment of antenatal emotional attachment: Development of a questionnaire instrument. Br J Med Psychol 1993,66(2):167-183.

13. Cranley MS: Development of a tool for the measurement of maternal attachment during pregnancy. Nurs Res 1981, 30(5):281-284.

14. Benoit D, Parker KCH, Zeanah CH: Mothers' Representations of Their Infants Assessed Prenatally: Stability and Association with Infants' Attachment Classifications. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997, 38(3):307-313.

15. Müller ME: Prenatal and Postnatal Attachment: A Modest Correlation. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs 1996, 25(2):161 -166.

16. Siddiqui A, Hägglöf B: Does maternal prenatal attachment predict postnatal mother-infant interaction? Early Hum Dev 2000, 59(1):13-25.

17. Theran SA, Levendosky AA, Bogat AG, Huth-Bocks AC: Stability and change in mothers' internal representations of their infants over time. Attachment Hum Dev 2005, 7(3):253-268.

18. Ainsworth MS, Bowlby J: An ethological approach to personality development. Am Psychol 1991, 46(4):333-341.

19. Bretherton I: The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Dev Psychol 1992, 28(5):759-775.

20. Zeanah CH, Smyke AT: In Attachment Disorders, Handbook of Infant Mental Health. 3rd edition. Edited by Zeanah CH. New York: Guilford Press; 2009.

21. Dayton CJ, Levendosky AA, Davidson WS, Bogat GA: The child as held in the mind of the mother: The influence of prenatal maternal representations on parenting behaviors. Infant Ment Health J 2010,31 (2):220-241.

22. Bruschweiler-Stern N: The Neonatal Moment of Meeting—Building the Dialogue, Strengthening the Bond. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin North Am 2009, 18(3):533-544.

23. Benoit D: Infant-parent attachment: Definition, types, antecedents, measurement and outcome. Paediatr Child Health 2004, 9(8):541.

24. Nicol-Harper R, Harvey AG, Stein A: Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behav Dev 2007, 30(1):161 -167.

25. Reck C, Hunt A, Fuchs T, Weiss R, Noon A, Moehler E, Downing G, Tronick EZ, Mundt C: Interactive regulation of affect in postpartum depressed mothers and their infants: an overview. Psychopathology 2004,37(6):272-280.

26. Singer LT, Fulton S, Davillier M, Koshy D, Salvator A, Baley JE: Effects of Infant Risk Status and Maternal Psychological Distress on Maternal-Infant Interactions During the First Year of Life. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2003,24(4):233-241.

27. Zeitlin D, Dhanjal T, Colmsee M: Maternal-foetal bonding: the impact of domestic violence on the bonding process between a mother and child. Arch Wom Ment Health 1999, 2(4):183-189.

28. Brockington IF, Aucamp HM, Fraser C: Severe disorders of the mother-infant relationship: definitions and frequency. Arch Wom Ment Health 2006, 9(5):243-251.

29. Wilson LM, Reid AJ, Midmer DK, Biringer A, Carroll JC, Stewart DE: Antenatal psychosocial risk factors associated with adverse postpartum family outcomes. CMAJ Can Med Assoc J 1996, 154(6):785.

30. Davis EP, Snidman N, Wadhwa PD, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, Sandman CA: Prenatal Maternal Anxiety and Depression Predict Negative Behavioral Reactivity in Infancy. Infancy 2004, 6(3):319-331.

31. Muir RC, Monaghan SM, Gilmore RJ, Clarkson JE, Crooks TJ, Egan TG: Predicting child abuse and neglect in New Zealand. Australas Psychiatr 1989, 23(2):255-260.

32. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R: Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatr Journal Ment Sci 1987, 150:782-786.

33. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE: Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970.

34. Arrindell WA, Ettema JHM: SCL-90 Symptom checklist: Handleiding bij een multidimensionele psychopathologie-indicator [SCL-90 Symptom checklist: Manual to a multidimensional psychopathology indicator]. Lisse: Swets Test Publishers; 2003.

35. Denollet J, Strik JJ, Lousberg R, Honig A: Recognizing increased risk of depressive comorbidity after myocardial infarction: Looking for 4 symptoms of anxiety-depression. Psychother Psychosom 2006, 75(6):346-352.

36. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R: A global measure of perceived stress. J Heal Soc Behav 1983, 24(4):385-396.

37. Vermulst AA, Gerris JRM: Quick Big Five persoonlijkheidstest [The Quick Big Five personality test]. Leeuwarden, NL: LDC Publishers; 2005.

38. Denollet J: DS14: Standard assessment of negative affectivity, social inhibition, and Type D personality. Psychosom Med 2005, 67(1):89-97.

39. Block J, Kremen AM: IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. J Personal Soc Psychol 1996, 70(2):349-361.

40. Holmes BM, Lyons-Ruth K: The relationship questionnaire-clinical version (RQ- CV): Introducing a profoundly-distrustful attachment style. Infant Ment Health J 2006, 27(3):310-325.

41. Koot HM: Handleiding bij de Vragenlijst voor Gezinsproblemen [Manual Questionnaire on Family Problems]. Rotterdam: Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Sophia Children's Hospital; 1997.

42. Raphael-Leff J: Facilitators and regulators: two approaches to mothering. Br J Med Psychol 1983, 56(4):379-390.

43. Bricker D, Squires J, Mounts L: Ages and Stages Questionnaire: A parent-completed, child-monitoring system. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes; 1995.

44. Squires J, Bricker D, Twombly E: Ages & Stages Questionnaires-Social-Emotional: A parent-completed, child-monitoring system for social-emotional behaviors. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc; 2002.

45. Bates JE, Freeland CA, Lounsbury ML: Measurement of infant difficultness. Child Dev 1979, 50(3):794-803.

46. Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ: Manual of the Infant-Toddler Social-Emotional Assessment. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 2000.

47. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS: Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) mannual, version 2.0. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 2002.

48. Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK: Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev 2003, 26(1):64-86.

49. Putnam SP, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK: Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev 2006, 29(3):386-401.

50. Gioia GA, Espy KA, Isquith PK: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Preschool Version (BRIEF-P). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2002.

51. Zeanah CH, Benoit D, Barton ML, Hirshberg L: Working model of the child interview coding manual.; 1996. Unpublished manuscript.

52. Van Bakel HJA, Vreeswijk CMJM, Maas AJBM: Pictorial and verbal representations of prenatal attachment [abstract]. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2009, 27:323.

53. NICHD, Early Child Care Research Network: Child Care and Mother-Child Interaction in the First 3 Years of Life. Dev Psychol 1999, 35(6):1399-1413.

54. Condon JT, Corkindale CJ: The assessment of parent-to-infant attachment: Development of a self-report questionnaire instrument. J Reprod Infant Psychol 1998, 16(1):57-76.

55. Condon JT, Corkindale CJ, Boyce P: Assessment of postnatal paternal-infant attachment: development of a questionnaire instrument. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2008, 26(3):195-210.

56. Brockington IF, Oates J, George S, Turner D, Vostanis P, Sullivan M, Loh C, Murdoch C: A Screening Questionnaire for mother-infant bonding disorders. Arch Wom Ment Health 2001, 3(4):133-140.

57. Stams GJJM, Hoeve M, Asscher JJ, Colonnesi C, Noom MJ, Polderman N, Kellaert-Knol M: Validation of the Attachment Difficulties Screening Inventory 2-5 Years Submitted for publication.; 2011.

58. de Brock AJLL, Vermulst AA, Gerris JRM, Abidin RR: NOSI. Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index. Handleiding experimentele versie. Lisse: Swets en Zeitlinger; 1992.

59. Barnes CR, Adamson-Macedo EN: Perceived Maternal Parenting Self-Efficacy (PMP S-E) tool: development and validation with mothers of hospitalized preterm neonates. J Adv Nurs 2007, 60(5):550-560.

60. Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Personal Soc Psychol 1986, 51 (6):1173.

61. Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.

62. Gatsonis C, Sampson AR: Multiple correlation: Exact power and sample size calculations. Psychol Bull 1989, 106(3):516.

doi:10.1186/1471 -2393-12-46

Cite this article as: Maas et al.: "Expectant Parents": Study protocol of a longitudinal study concerning prenatal (risk) factors and postnatal infant development, parenting, and parent-infant relationships. BMC

Pregnancy and Childbirth 2012 12:46.