Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
journal homepage: http://www.schizrescognition.com/
A novel, online social cognitive training program for young adults with schizophrenia: A pilot study
Mor Nahum aÄ*, Melissa Fisherc,d, Rachel Loewyc, Gina Poelkec,d, Joseph Ventura e, Keith H. Nuechterlein e,f, Christine I. Hooker g, Michael F. Green e,h, Michael M. Merzenich a, Sophia Vinogradovc,d
a Posit Science, San Francisco, CA, USA
b Department of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA c Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA d San Francisco Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Francisco, CA, USA e Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA f Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA g Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA h VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, USA
ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT
Background: Pervasive social cognition deficits are evident early in the course of schizophrenia and are directly linked to functional outcome, making them an important target for intervention. Here, we tested the feasibility of use, and initiated the evaluation of efficacy, of a novel, neuroplasticity-based online training program (SocialVille) in young adults with schizophrenia.
Methods: Schizophrenia patients (n = 17) completed 24 hours of online SocialVille game play either from home or at a clinic, over a 6-10 week period. We examined training feasibility, gains on the SocialVille exercises relative to matched healthy controls (n = 17), and changes on measures of social cognition, social functioning, global functioning and motivation.
Results: Subjects adhered to training requirements, and rated SocialVille in the medium to high range in satisfaction, enjoyment, and ease of use. Subjects demonstrated significant, large improvements on the speeded SocialVille tasks, and small to moderate improvements on the working memory tasks. Post-training performance on the SocialVille tasks were similar to initial performance of the healthy controls. Subjects also showed improvements on standard measures of social cognition, social functioning, and motivation. No improvements were recorded for emotion recognition indices of the MSCEIT, or on quality of life scales. Conclusion: This study provides an initial proof of concept for online social cognition training in schizophrenia. This form of training demonstrated feasibility and resulted in within-subject gains in social functioning and motivation. This pilot study represents a first step towards validating this training approach; randomized controlled trials, now underway, are designed to confirm and extend these findings.
© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
CrossMark
Article history:
Received 24 October 2013
Received in revised form 22 January 2014
Accepted 24 January 2014
Available online 12 March 2014
Keywords: Social cognition Early psychosis First episode Computerized training Cognitive remediation
1. Introduction
In recent years, special emphasis has been given to the need for early intervention in schizophrenia (Insel, 2010; McGorry, 2011; Wykes et al., 2007). It is now acknowledged that the early phase of psychotic illness is crucial in terms of the emergence of a range of cognitive deficits that have prognostic implications (Bartholomeusz and Allott, 2012; Birchwood et al., 1990) - and that early intervention can potentially prevent further worsening of symptoms and improve
Abbreviations: SZ, Schizophrenia; HC, Healthy Control.
* Corresponding author. Mor Nahum, Posit Science, 77 Geary St., Suite 303, San Francisco, CA 94108. Tel.: +1 415 269 2425.
E-mail address: mor.nahum@positscience.com (M. Nahum).
2215-0013/$ - see front matter © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/! 0.1016/j.scog.2014.01.003
functioning. An important target for early intervention is the domain of social cognition, the mental operations that underlie understanding, interpretation and perception of social information (Fiske and Taylor, 1991 ). Severe social cognition deficits, often comparable to those seen in chronic patients, have been repeatedly documented in early-phase schizophrenia (e.g. Bertrand et al., 2007; Green et al., 2012a; Williams et al., 2008). These deficits span the domains of affect perception (e.g. Edwards et al., 2001), social cue perception (including gaze perception, e.g. Hooker and Park, 2005; Rosse et al., 1994; Tso et al., 2013; Tso et al., 2012), theory of mind (ToM; e.g. Bertrand et al., 2007) and attributional style (e.g. Humphreys and Barrowclough, 2006). Importantly, social cognition deficits have been strongly associated with poor functional outcome in schizophrenia (e.g. Fett et al., 2010). Specifically, affect recognition and social perception have been each linked with community functioning, social problem solving
and social skills (Couture et al., 2006; Irani et al., 2012); ToM, as well as affect perception and social perception, have been found to mediate the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome (Addington et al., 2006a, b; Billeke and Aboitiz, 2013; Brekke et al., 2005; Couture et al., 2011; Fett etal., 2010; Sergi et al., 2006).
Surprisingly, however, only a few studies to date have examined the direct effects of social cognition training in young adult or early psychosis patients (Bartholomeusz et al., 2013; Eack et al., 2007; Eack et al., 2009), and none have evaluated a computerized intervention. Studies testing the effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy, a computer-based cognitive training with group-based social skills training (Eack et al., 2007,2011; Eack et al., 2009) and of SCIT, a social cognitive group intervention (Combs et al., 2007; Penn et al., 2005) in first episode patients report promising effects on neurocognitive, social cognitive and functional outcome measures. However, these encouraging outcomes are limited by the practicality of applying these treatments in many clinical settings, given long treatment durations, the need for a trained clinician team, and the necessity of organizing patient groups for program delivery.
Computer-based training has the potential to overcome these limitations by allowing individualized treatment that can be more flexibly performed from home, and at a considerable cost savings (Ventura et al., 2013). A few recent studies (Frommann et al., 2003; Hooker et al., 2012; Hooker et al., 2013; Russell et al., 2006; Sacks et al., 2013; Wolwer et al., 2005) tested computerized training of facial affect recognition and mental state decoding in chronic patients, with promising results in emotion perception, management and social functioning (Kurtz and Richardson, 2012). These results in chronic patients show that improving social cognition and social skills using a single-user, computer-based intervention is feasible and is potentially beneficial. However, it is unclear whether younger patients would comply with computerized social cognitive training, and with the requirement to individually train from home for several hours each week over a several week long therapeutic epoch.
Here, we examined the feasibility and initial efficacy of a new, online training program (SocialVille by Brain Plasticity Institute of Posit Science; Nahum et al., 2013a), in young adults with schizophrenia. SocialVille aims to treat social cognition deficits using the principles of neuroplasticity-based learning (Merzenich, 2013; Nahum et al., 2013b), by targeting the impaired brain systems underlying social cognition rather than the impaired social behaviors per-se that are targeted by molar social skills training approaches. The SocialVille exercises aim to improve efficiency of stimulus representation and processing speed in the specific neural systems that underlie social cognition, and have been shown to function abnormally in schizophrenia (e.g. Crossley et al., 2009). The rationale behind this approach was successfully applied to address general cognitive deficits in chronic schizophrenia (e.g. Fisher et al., 2009; Subramaniam et al., 2012): that directly strengthening the fidelity of representations of socially-relevant information in the brain should improve an individual's social behavior. The SocialVille exercises employ psychophysical principles of training and exploit implicit learning mechanisms: they continuously adapt in difficulty, based on individual performance. The user learns through tasks that involve many socially-relevant stimulus examples while given feedback on correct and incorrect discriminations. These neuroplasticity-based principles, which provided the basis for the construction of SocialVille, have been recently summarized in a review paper (see Nahum et al., 2013b for full details) (Fig. 1).
To assess feasibility, we predicted that at least 75% of enrolled participants would complete the training and that they would report satisfaction with the exercises with ratings of over 4 on the 7-point Likert scale items. We measured learning rates by assessing improvement on the SocialVille exercises. We hypothesized that participants would show improvements on the trained exercises, as well as learning generalization to proximal social cognition measures. We
explored whether more distal measures of functioning and motivation would improve with this form of computerized training.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
Schizophrenia (SZ) subjects (n = 17) and matched healthy controls (HC) (n = 17) completed the study protocol in two sites at university-based early psychosis programs at the University of California, San Francisco and Los Angeles (Table 1). SZ subjects met the following inclusion/exclusion criteria: Diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizophreniform, or schizoaffective disorder via the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV; First et al., 1996); good general physical health; age 18-31 years; Premorbid IQ > 70 (estimated using WTAR; Wechsler, 2001); No neurological disorder or history of traumatic brain injury; No substance dependence or serious substance use in the past 6 months. SZ Symptom severity at baseline was assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1987). We note that nine subjects were within 2 years of psychosis onset, seven more within 5 years, and that only one was more than five years from onset (7.2 years). All subjects had achieved clinical stability (outpatient status for at least 3 months prior to study participation) and were stable on psychiatric medications for at least one month prior to study onset.
UCSF site participants (n = 10) were either active or former patients at the early psychosis clinic (EPC), which provides medication management and some psychotherapy; for the duration of the study, 4 were receiving no treatment, 3 were receiving outside treatment and 3 were seen at the EPC. UCLA participants were enrolled in the Aftercare Program protocol, and all were receiving psychosocial treatment, medication, and case management there. None of the UCLA participants were in other cognitive remediation training during their SocialVille training.
HC subjects (status verified through the SCID-Nonpatient edition) were recruited to match the SZ cohort at a group level in terms of age, gender and education (Table 1).
2.2. Study Procedures
SZ subjects: Following informed consent, eligible participants underwent baseline assessments. Subjects from the UCSF clinic (n = 10) were then loaned laptop computers, given logins and asked to complete the intervention from home. The UCLA clinic
Table 1
Sample characteristics of the patient and healthy control groups.
Variable Schizophrenia (SZ)(n = 17) Healthy control (HC)(n = 17) Analysesa X2 df P
Male/female 13/4 12/5 .151 1 .69
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F df P
Age, years 23.8 (±3.2) 23.6 (±3.6) .20 32 .84
Education, years 12.8 (±1.7) 13.5 (±1.5) -1.28 32 .21
IQ (WTAR) 101.2 (±16.2) 110.7 (±11.3) -1.95 32 .06
Time since psychosis 38.5 (±23.9) - - - -
onset, months
PANSSb
Positive 13.1 (± 1.2) - - - -
Negative 16.2 (± 1.4) - - - -
General psychopathology 27.8 (±2.1) - - - -
TOTAL 57.1 (±4.1) - - - -
Medication (chlorpromazine 281.5 (±31.1) N/A - - -
equivalent)
Total training times are marked in bold type. a Independent samples t-test or chi-square analysis for categorical data. b Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.
subjects (n = 7) came into the clinic twice a week to complete the intervention there, as they were already coming to the clinic twice each week anyway. Subjects from both sites were asked to complete 24 hours of training with the SocialVille online program (1-2 hours per day, 2-5 days per week for 6-12 weeks). Following training completion, subjects completed the post-training assessment battery.
HC subjects: following consent and initial screening, participants completed the SocialVille exercise-based assessments only, i.e. one block of every SocialVille social cognitive exercise (see details below). Subjects received monetary compensation for participating in the study.
Assessments were conducted by psychology practicum graduate students (UCSF site) or by case managers and research assistants (UCLA site); assessors were not directly paid by Posit Science for their services.
2.3. Social Cognition Training intervention (SocialVille)
(ii) Emotional prosody identification: The performance-based Prosody Identification Test (PRO1D; Russ, 2008) assessed subjects' accuracy and reaction time in identifying the emotional prosody in a series of sentences.
(iii) Emotion and social perception: Assessed the perceiving emotions and managing emotions branches of the performance-based Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCE1T; Mayer et al., 2003) battery.
2.4.4. Functioning
(i) Functioning: the clinician-rated Global Functioning: Social and Role Scales assessed social and occupational functioning (Cornblattet al., 2007).
(ii) Social functioning was also assessed with the self-report Social Functioning Scale (SFS; Birchwood et al., 1990).
(iii) Quality of life: The clinician-rated Quality of Life Scale-Abbreviated (QLS; Bilker et al., 2003).
SocialVille is an online program designed to treat social cognition deficits in schizophrenia. It consists of 19 computerized exercises targeting speed and accuracy of neural functions dedicated to processing of social information (e.g. Nahum et al., 2013a; Nahum et al., 2013b). Specifically, the SocialVille exercises target the social cognitive domains of affect perception (both visual and vocal), social cue perception, ToM and self-referential processing (see Table 2). A full description of the SocialVille program is provided in Supplemental Document 1. Each SocialVille session consisted of 6 exercise blocks, each taking about 10 minutes to complete.
2.4. Outcome measures
2.4.1. SocialVille training program feasibility and ease of use:
(i) Attrition rate: percentage of subjects who actually completed the 24 hours of training
(ii) Adherence with training schedules: number of training sessions/week
(iii) SocialVille program rating: upon completion of training, SZ subjects were asked to complete a questionnaire in which they rated enjoyment, satisfaction, ease of use, program attractiveness, frustration level and program security concerns using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree through 7 = strongly agree).
2.4.2. Social Cognition Learning: SocialVille Exercise-based Assessments
Performance levels on the SocialVille exercises (i.e. social cognition abilities) were assessed at baseline (SZ and HC participants) and post-training (SZ only) using the exercise-based assessments embedded in the SocialVille program. Each assessment is a single 'block' of each exercise (except for two exercises, see Table 2), for which performance threshold is determined using an adaptive staircase algorithm (e.g. Levitt, 1971) within 5-15 minutes. A full description of the exercise-based assessments is included in Supplemental Document 1.
For each exercise-based assessment, we derived normalized (Z) scores relative to the mean score of the HC group on that assessment. This was done to allow the creation of composite scores by summing assessment scores across domains, and to more easily visually depict test scores across a variety of scales (e.g. reaction time in ms, number of correct items, % correct, etc.).
The remainder of the outcome measures were administered to the SZ group only:
2.4.3. Social cognition outcome measures
(i) Facial memory: The performance-based Penn Facial Memory Test (Gur et al., 2001) assessed immediate and delayed (following 30 minutes) memory of neutral faces.
Table 2
SocialVille exercises and total time spent on each exercise during 24 hour course of training.
Exercise name
Exercise description
Time (hours)
Speeded exercises
S1 Gaze ID* A speeded gaze identification task 1
(select the peripheral object)
S Gaze Match* A speeded gaze matching task 1
(match gaze direction of target face)
S Face Match* A speeded face matching task 1.2
S Face Emotion ID* A speeded facial emotion 2
identification task (stills)
S Face Emotion ID (Clips)* A speeded facial emotion 1.2
identification task (video clips)
S Face Emotion Match A speeded facial emotion matching 1.6 task (stills)
S Valence Match* Delayed match of the valence of a 1 target picture
Facial Emotion CPT3* Withhold response for neutral faces, 1
press for emotional faces
Emotion Maintenance* Select the smiling face when faces 1 keep changing
Facial Affect ToM* Select the correct affective (facial) 1.2
response in a given situation
Total time on speeded exercises 12.2
WM2 exercises
WM Facial Emotions Match pairs of facial emotion cards 1 in a WM task
WM Facial Emotion (clips)* Match pairs of facial emotions and 1.5
labels in a WM task
WM Vocal Emotions Match pairs of vocal emotion cards 1 in a WM task
Faces Span Arrange the faces in the order they 1.3 were presented
Facial Emotion Span Arrange the facial emotions in the 1
order they were presented
Face Stories Span* Memorize sequences of faces and 1
personal facts on the faces
Total time on WM exercises 6.8
Other exercises Vocal Emotion ID
Social StoriesNM Vocal Affect ToMNM
Identify the prosody of the sentence 1.2 with the neutral content
Answer questions regarding social 2.5 interactions in a segmented story
Select the correct prosody 1.5 (vocal affect)
response in a given situation
Total time on other exercises 5.2
Total training times are marked in bold type.
1 S: Speeded Task
2 WM: Working Memory.
3 CPT: Continuous Performance Task.
* A task for which there was a significant improvement in threshold following training; NMA task for which threshold performance was not measured.
Fig. 1. Examples of the SocialVille tasks. Top: Face Perception ('S Face Match') exercise. Once the user clicks on the 'start' button, a face appears for a short duration, followed by a visual mask for 500 ms. Then, an array of faces appears, and the user should select the target face. A feedback is provided following the selection. The duration of presentation of the target face is adaptively determined based on the user's responses. Middle and Bottom: Gaze Perception ('S Gaze Match'; middle panel) and Emotion Identification ('S Face Emotion ID'; bottom panel). The same trial structure as described above is employed for these exercises as well, but here the user needs to either match the gaze direction of the target face ('S Gaze Match') or identify the facial expression.
2.4.5. Self-report motivation measures
(i) Motivation: The self-report Behavioral Inhibition Scale/Behavioral Activation Scale (BIS/BAS; Carver and White, 1994) was used to assess two motivational systems, each with a unique neural signature, that regulate approach and withdrawal behavior.
(ii) Reward sensitivity was assessed with the self-report Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS; Gard et al., 2006), which assesses anticipatory pleasure and consummatory pleasure. Anticipatory pleasure is closely linked to motivation and goal-directed behavior while consummatory pleasure is associated with satiation (i.e. wanting versus liking). Research indicates that schizophrenia patients have a deficit in anticipatory pleasure, but normal consummately pleasure (Gard et al., 2007).
2.5. Data analysis
All variables were screened and normally distributed after winsorising of outlying values. Group differences in gender were tested using a chi-square test. Independent Samples t-tests tested for group differences in age, education level, IQ and the exercise-based assessments.
Paired samples two-tailed t-tests were used to examine whether SZ participants made significant gains on the training exercises and to test changes from baseline to post-training on all other outcome measures. Two-tailed tests with no correction for multiple comparisons were used because gains were hypothesized and because of the preliminary nature of the study.
3. Results
3.1. Clinical and demographic data
Demographic and clinical data is summarized in Table 1. The HC and SZ groups did not differ significantly in gender or education. As is typical with this population, the groups differed slightly in IQ, with the difference approaching statistical significance (F(1,32) = —1.95; p = .06).
3.2. SocialVille program feasibility
3.2.1. Attrition Rate
Twenty-two (22) SZ participants were recruited (UCSF: n = 13; UCLA: n = 9). Seventeen completed the study. Five subjects (3 from UCSF and 2 from UCLA) dropped out at initial phases (4 before being given the training laptop and one after completing 2 training sessions). The reasons provided for dropping out were as follows: due to increased stress at work/school (n = 2), hospitalization (n = 1), boredom (n = 1) and unknown reason (n = 1). Attrition rate was not different for the two sites (23% and 22% at UCSF and UCLA, respectively).
3.2.2. Compliance with online SocialVille training
Adherence to the online training schedule. Subjects were asked to complete 2-5 one-hour sessions of training per week for 6-12 weeks (24 sessions total). On average, subjects took 8.1 ± 2.5 weeks to complete training (range: 5-12 weeks), with only 4 subjects
Fig. 2. Normalized SocialVille Exercise-based Assessment Data. Averaged ( + SEM) Z score changes on the 'speeded' (A), WM (B), and composites (C) SocialVille exercises following training. The composites were derived as averages across all exercises in the same category; the 'total' composite is the average across all exercises (excluding prosody). Note the different scales on the three panels. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .005.
taking more than 10 weeks to complete the program. The average number of weekly sessions completed was 3.15 ± 0.8.
SocialVille program rating. Following training, subjects rated their satisfaction in playing SocialVille on a 7-point Likert scale. The following averaged ratings were obtained: (a) Satisfaction rating ('I felt satisfied after the training') of 5 ± 1.9 (5 corresponds to 'somewhat agree'); (b) Program clarity rating ('the exercise instructions and tutorials were easy to understand') of 6 ± 1 (6 corresponds to 'mostly agree'); (c) Ease of navigation rating ('the program was easy to navigate') of 5.8 ± 1.3; (d) Ease of use rating ('the program makes it quick and easy for me to start playing each day') of 4.7 ± 2 and ease of fit into daily schedule ('the program easily fits into my daily schedule') of 4.5 ± 1.8; (e) Attractiveness rating ('the program graphics were attractive') of 4.5 ± 1.9; (f) Program usage difficulty ("the program was difficult to use') was rated as 3.06 ± 2.1 ('somewhat disagree'); (g) Security concerns of using the online program ('I was worried about the security of my log-in account') were 2.3 ± 1.9 (corresponding to 'mostly disagree' with the statement).
3.3. Social Cognition Learning: SocialVille Exercise-based Assessments
For 17 of the 19 SocialVille exercises, we calculated a normalized z-score for the SZ group relative to the HC baseline performance on each exercise. For two exercises, no thresholds were derived due to a data recording error, hence they were not included in the normalized composites (see Table 2). Results are summarized in Fig. 2, in Supplementary Figure 1 and in Supplementary Table 1. On the 'speeded' SocialVille exercises (i.e. tasks that required speeded processing of stimulus on every trial, Fig. 2A), significant z-score changes from baseline to post-training were evident on all but one of the exercises (two
were at trend-level). Following training, patients' performance on these tasks was similar to that of the initial performance of the HC group (see Supplementary Figure 1). On the 6 WM-based SocialVille exercises, improvements were evident on 5 of the exercises, however only one of these reached statistical significance (Figure 2B). We further derived composite scores for the speeded tasks, the WM tasks and a total composite score. Significant z-score changes were seen on all composite scores (speeded: t(16) = — 6.9, p < .0001; WM: t(16) = — 2.6, p < .02; Total: t(16) = — 7.4, p < .0001). The pre-to post-test gain on the speeded task composite score was large (1.15 SD), while the gain in the WM composite score was small to moderate (0.45 SD).
3.4. Social cognition outcome measures
Outcome measures data for the study are summarized in Table 3.
(i) Facial memory: the median RT for correct responses of the Penn Facial Memory test significantly decreased for both immediate and delayed recall (Fig. 3B).
(ii) Prosody identification: following training, PROID accuracy showed a non-significant tendency to increase and median RT significantly decreased (Fig. 3A).
(iii) Emotion and social perception: no significant changes were seen on the perceiving emotions or managing emotions sub-scales of the MSCEIT test.
3.5. Functioning
Subjects showed a significant increase in GFS's social functioning, and no significant change in role functioning (see Fig. 4A). On the SFS there was a trend-level increase on the interpersonal communication
Table 3
Scores on the study outcome measures before (baseline) and after (post-training) 24 hours of SocialVille training.
Outcome measure Baseline Post-training Stats
Mean (±SD) Mean (±SD) t p Effect size
Emotional Prosody ID (PROID)
# Correct responses 40.06 (±1.87) 42.9 (±1.96) 1.8 0.09 0.37
Median RT 3940 (±129) 3540 (±144) 2.94 < .01 0.71
Facial Memory (Penn Test) - immediate recall
# Correct responses 30.06 (±1.1) 31.6 (±1.1) 1.4 0.16 0.22
Median RT 2025 (±164) 1679 (±116) 3.14 < .007 0.6
Facial Memory (Penn Test) - delayed recall
# Correct responses 30.5 (±1.1) 31.1 (±1.4) .49 0.63 0.11
Median RT 1870 (±217) 1415 (± 87) 2.43 < 0.03 0.73
MSCEIT
Perceiving Emotions SS1 107.3 (±3.3) 105.1 (±4.1) .63 0.54 0.12
Managing Emotions SS 91.9 (±3.7) 94.1 (±4.9) .72 0.42 0.15
Global Functioning Scale (GFS)
GFS: Role Current SS 5.2 (±0.7) 5.5 (±0.6) .85 0.41 0.085
GFS: Social Current SS 5.8 (±0.47) 6.6 (±0.43) 2.5 <0.03 0.4
Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
SFS: Interpersonal Communication SS 113.2 (±3.9) 119.1 (±4.6) 1.6 0.12 0.37
Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scale (BIS/BAS)
BIS Total 3.11 (±0.12) 2.96 (±0.14) 2.2 0.04 0.28
BAS Reward Responsiveness 3.24 (±0.14) 3.14 (±0.18) 1.07 0.39 0.17
BAS Drive 2.52 (±0.18) 2.71 (±0.2) 1.76 0.09 0.24
BAS Fun Seeking 2.94 (±0.13) 2.99 (±0.18) .70 1 0
The Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS)
TEPS: Anticipatory SS 38.06 (±2.6) 41.1 (±2.5) 2.29 <0.04 0.28
TEPS: Consummatory SS 33 (±2.2) 32.3 (±2.2) .54 0.6 0.07
Quality of Life Scale Total 3.29 (±0.3) 3.26 (±0.3) .17 0.86 0.02
Significant or trend-level p values are marked in bold type. 1 SS: Subscale.
subscale (Fig. 4B), and no significant change on the other SFS subscales. There were no significant changes on the Quality of Life Scale.
3.6. Motivation/reward sensitivity
(i) Motivation: we found a significant decrease on the Behavioral Inhibition Total scale and a trend-level increase in the Behavioral Activation-Drive subscale (Fig. 4C). Changes on the two other BIS/BAS subscales were non-significant (see Table 3).
(ii) Reward Sensitivity: we found an increase on the TEPS Anticipatory Pleasure subscale and no significant change on the Consummately Pleasure subscale (Fig. 4D).
4. Discussion
We tested the feasibility and preliminary efficacy of SocialVille, a new online training program targeting deficits in social cognitive processing speed and working memory (WM) in young adults with schizophrenia. Following 24 hours of training performed over a 6-12
week period, we found: relatively high adherence with the training requirements, satisfaction with the exercises; significant pre- to posttraining improvements on the SocialVille social cognition exercise composite scores; significant improvements on proximal measures of social cognition (prosody identification and facial memory); and significant improvements on social functioning, motivation and reward sensitivity.
These findings suggest that an online training approach is feasible in early psychosis, and that social cognitive deficits may be addressed early in the course of schizophrenia. Given the importance of early intervention (Birchwood et al., 1998; Marshall and Rathbone, 2011), and the link between social cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia (Fett et al., 2010; Sergi et al., 2007) this study provides an initial scalable way of targeting a set of critical core deficits. Although promising, the results are preliminary and not case-controlled. An appropriately powered, randomized controlled trial is required to determine whether these effects are replicable.
To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to demonstrate the feasibility of online social cognitive training in young adults with
Fig. 3. Outcome measures data. Averaged performance (±SEM) before (blue bars) and after (red bars) SocialVille training. (A) Median reaction time (RT, in ms) for correct responses on the PROID task; (C) Facial recognition test (Penn) data: median reaction time (RT, in ms) for immediate (left) and delayed (right) recall of faces. *p values and Cohen's d values are shown; n.s. non-significant.
Fig. 4. Outcome measures data. Averaged performance (+SEM) before (blue bars) and after (red bars) training. (A) Global Functioning Scale (GFS), Role Current and Social Current subscales; (B) Social Functioning Scale (SFS), interpersonal communication and prosocial subscales; (C) Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation (BIS/BAS) test, BIS total and BAS drive subscales. (D) Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS), anticipatory (left) and consummatory (right) subscales.
schizophrenia (see recent review in Bartholomeusz and Allott, 2012). Two other studies tested the feasibility of social cognitive training in early psychosis, both using group-based interventions administered in clinic (SCIT, Bartholomeusz et al., 2013; CET, Eack et al., 2007). CET uses computerized 'cold' cognition training with group-based social skills training. SCIT is a group-based therapy as well, comprised of 18 sessions focused on three phases of understanding emotions, social cognitive biases, and integration, in which trainees practice the acquired social skills in everyday situations (Penn et al., 2005). These two interventions, although including some computer-assisted parts, are administered in a therapist-instructed group format. Here, we show the feasibility of a fully-computerized intervention, which can be completed either at home or at the clinic, using internet-connected computers. Gains were obtained after a relatively short period of treatment (less than 12 weeks). The attrition rate in our study is similar to other training studies in young clinical populations (e.g., Marshall and Rathbone, 2011). Moreover, the high compliance with the training schedule indicates that this form of individual online training is feasible with young adults, comparable to clinic-based group training. We note that at-home participants needed weekly phone-calls and/or emails to reliably adhere to training requirements, thus clinical staff involvement in the form of solution-focused conversations was still required. Future studies will determine whether combining our computer-based training with psychosocial interventions (e.g. social skills groups) enhances generalization. Still, our study provides the first demonstration of a potentially highly-scalable form of treatment that could be easily used in practically every household or clinical facility equipped with an internet connection, thus providing treatment options to under-resourced areas and to patients who are unable or unwilling to come in to the clinic.
Study participants made larger gains on the 'speeded' SocialVille composite (i.e. processing speed of social information), whereas improvements on the 'WM' composite (i.e. WM manipulations of social information) were smaller. The fact that large pre-post gains were
found in processing speed is encouraging: recent 'cold cognition' studies consider speed of processing deficits among the largest cognitive impairments in recent-onset schizophrenia (Mesholam-Gately et al., 2009; Milev et al., 2005) and hence an important target for early intervention. While speed of processing during "cold cognition" tasks and social cognitive tasks may be quite different, our results suggest that speed-of-processing deficits of social information are evident in early psychosis, and that training drives improvements in this fundamental ability.
Although there are currently no rigorous studies of computerized social cognition training in early schizophrenia, our outcomes are comparable to recent reports of social cognition training in chronic schizophrenia (see Kurtz and Richardson, 2012 for review; Lindenmayer et al., 2013), and further strengthen the notion that social cognition is linked to social functioning and functional outcome in schizophrenia (Billeke and Aboitiz, 2013; Brekke et al., 2005). Interestingly, while SocialVille includes training on facial affect, we did not find significant changes on the MSCEIT emotion perception and management subscales. However, our findings still suggest that subjects improved on emotion recognition abilities, as is evident by improvements on SocialVille emotion exercises and on vocal affect recognition. We hypothesize that this negative result is likely due to the focus of SocialVille on processing speed, while affect perception and management is measured differently in the MSCEIT (see similar arguments in Roberts et al., 2006). The short duration of training relative to that included in other studies (e.g. Eack et al., 2007; Eack et al., 2009) might have also contributed to the lack of improvement on the MSCEIT.
Following training, participants improved on motivation and reward sensitivity: participants showed decreased behavioral inhibition and increased drive, as well as increased anticipatory pleasure. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of changes in motivation following cognitive training in early schizophrenia. Interestingly, motivation is generally considered a stable trait in healthy
individuals, not subject to change. A few recent reports (e.g. Gard et al., 2009; Green et al., 2012b) have shown that motivation plays a significant and mediating role between neurocognition, social cognition and functional outcome. Our preliminary finding that motivation can be enhanced with social cognitive training provides strong support for this model, and further stresses the importance of targeting social cognition in schizophrenia (see also Choi and Medalia, 2010).
Our study had several limitations. These include the small sample size, the lack of a control group, and the fact that participants were provided remuneration for participation in the study. Further, we cannot rule out practice effects or non-specific effects of study participation. These factors all limit our ability to attribute improvements to the SocialVille training itself. Furthermore, since the main goal of the study was to establish feasibility in early schizophrenia patients, no general cognition and symptom outcome measures were included. Future, well-controlled studies are required to further establish the efficacy of computerized social cognitive training administered with no additional neurocognitive training (see discussion in Pinkham and Harvey, 2013). Also, the results of our pilot study do not rule out the possibility that improvements are driven by non-specific effects of training such as increased attention span or general improved processing speed. Still, results from several recent studies (e.g. Fisher et al., 2009; Lindenmayer et al., 2013; Sacks et al., 2013) imply that non-social cognitive training does not improve social cognition and social function. Finally, we note that there is currently no consensus on the best social cognition outcome measures to be used in intervention studies, as many of them are considered to have poor psychometric characteristics (see Pinkham et al., 2013). Future studies should consider applying additional or different outcome measures, given new psychometric information on outcome measures (see, for example, Green et al., 2013; Kern et al., 2013).
We conclude that SocialVille is a promising intervention which is feasible and resulted in initial positive outcomes in social cognition, social functioning, and motivation in young individuals with schizophrenia. Given the importance of early intervention, and the lack of effective treatment options, there is a clear need for effective, scalable treatments. Future randomized controlled trials will determine whether these preliminary findings are replicable and are needed to discover the 'active ingredients' of training that allow for learning to transfer to everyday functioning.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2014.01.003.
Role of Funding Source
The study was funded by a National Institute of Mental Health SBIR Grant (1R43MH091793-01A1) to author M.N.
Contributors
Author M.N. developed the training program, designed the study, supervised the study, conducted data analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Author M.F. helped supervise the UCSF site study, conducted data analyses and wrote the manuscript. Author R.L. supervised the trial and managed patient recruitment and participation. Author G.P. supervised the assessments and outcome measures. Authors J.V. and K.N. supervised the UCLA site study and provided input on the development of the training program. Authors C.H. and MFG provided input on the development of the training program. Author M.M. helped develop the training program and design the study. Author S.V. helped develop the training program, designed the study and supervised the UCSF site study. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of interest
The social cognitive training software used in this study (SocialVille) was developed by Posit Science. Dr. Nahum is a paid employee of Posit Science and was the main developer of the program. In addition, she received SBIR grant from NIMH to develop and test the SocialVille software. Dr. Merzenich is the founder and CSO of Posit Science. Drs. Vinogradov, Hooker, Green and Ventura are paid consultants to Posit Science and were all involved in the construction of the training program. Dr. Nuechterlein is an unpaid consultant to Posit Science, and holds research grants from Janssen Scientific Affairs and Genentech. He serves as a consultant to Otsuka and Genentech. Dr. Vinogradov serves on advisory boards for Genentech, Envivo, and Hoffman-LaRoche. Dr. Green reports having been a consultant to Abbott Laboratories (AbbVie), Biogen, DSP and Roche. He is a member of the scientific board for Mnemosyne, and has received research funds from Amgen. Dr. Ventura has received research support from Brain Plasticity Inc. (a company merged with Posit Science) and from Janssen Scientific Affairs. Dr. Loewy has received research funding from Genentech.
Drs. Fisher and Poelke report no conflicts of interest.
References
Addington, J., Saeedi, H., Addington, D., 2006a. Facial affect recognition: A mediator between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis? Schizophr. Res. 85 (1-3), 142-150.
Addington, J., Saeedi, H., Addington, D., 2006b. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br. J. Psychiatry: J. Mental. Sci. 189, 373-378.
Bartholomeusz, C.F., Allott, K., 2012. Neurocognitive and social cognitive approaches for improving functional outcome in early psychosis: Theoretical considerations and current state of evidence. Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2012, 815315.
Bartholomeusz, C.F., Allott, K., Killackey, E., Liu, P., Wood, S.J., Thompson, A., 2013. Social cognition training as an intervention for improving functional outcome in first-episode psychosis: A feasibility study. Early Interv. Psychiatry 7 (4), 421-426.
Bertrand, M.C., Sutton, H., Achim, A.M., Malla, A.K., Lepage, M., 2007. Social cognitive impairments in first episode psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 95 (1-3), 124-133.
Bilker, W.B., Brensinger, C., Kurtz, M.M., Kohler, C., Gur, R.C., Siegel, S.J., Gur, R.E., 2003. Development of an abbreviated schizophrenia quality of life scale using a new method. Neuropsychopharmacology 28 (4), 773-777.
Billeke, P., Aboitiz, F., 2013. Social cognition in schizophrenia: From social stimuli processing to social engagement. Front. Psychiatry 4,4.
Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Cochrane, R., Wetton, S., Copestake, S., 1990. The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. Br. J. Psychiatry 157, 853-859.
Birchwood, M., Todd, P., Jackson, C., 1998. Early intervention in psychosis. The critical period hypothesis. Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 172 (33), 53-59.
Brekke, J., Kay, D.D., Lee, K.S., Green, M.F., 2005. Biosocial pathways to functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 80 (2-3), 213-225.
Carver, C.S., White, T.L., 1994. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67,1126-1133.
Choi, J., Medalia, A., 2010. Intrinsic motivation and learning in a schizophrenia spectrum sample. Schizophr. Res. 118 (1-3), 12-19.
Combs, D.R., Adams, S.D., Penn, D.L., Roberts, D., Tiegreen, J., Stem, P., 2007. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophr. Res. 91 (1-3), 112-116.
Cornblatt, B.A., Auther, A.M., Niendam, T., Smith, C.W., Zinberg, J., Bearden, C.E., Cannon, T.D., 2007. Preliminary findings for two new measures of social and role functioning in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 33 (3), 688-702.
Couture, S.M., Granholm, E.L., Fish, S.C., 2011. A path model investigation of neuro-cognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 125 (2-3), 152-160.
Couture, S.M., Penn, D.L., Roberts, D.L., 2006. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophr. Bull. 32 (Suppl 1), S44-S63.
Crossley, N.A., Mechelli, A., Fusar-Poli, P., Broome, M.R., Matthiasson, P., Johns, L.C., Bramon, E., Valmaggia, L., Williams, S.C., McGuire, P.K., 2009. Superior temporal lobe dysfunction and frontotemporal dysconnectivity in subjects at risk of psychosis and in first-episode psychosis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 (12), 4129-4137.
Eack, S.M., Greenwald, D.P., Hogarty, S.S., Cooley, S.J., DiBarry, A.L., Montrose, D.M., Keshavan, M.S., 2009. Cognitive enhancement therapy for early-course schizophrenia: Effects of a two-year randomized controlled trial. Psychiatr. Serv. 60 (11), 1468-1476.
Eack, S.M., Hogarty, G.E., Greenwald, D.P., Hogarty, S.S., Keshavan, M.S., 2007. Cognitive enhancement therapy improves emotional intelligence in early course schizophrenia: Preliminary effects. Schizophr. Res. 89 (1-3), 308-311.
Eack, S.M., Hogarty, G.E., Greenwald, D.P., Hogarty, S.S., Keshavan, M.S., 2011. Effects of cognitive enhancement therapy on employment outcomes in early schizophrenia: Results from a two-year randomized trial. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 21 (1), 32-42.
Edwards, J., Pattison, P.E., Jackson, H.J., Wales, R.J., 2001. Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 48 (2-3), 235-253.
Fett, A.K., Viechbauer, W., et al., 2010. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35 (3), 573-588.
First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J.B.W., 1996. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV). American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Fisher, M., Holland, C., Merzenich, M.M., Vinogradov, S., 2009. Using neuroplasticity-based auditory training to improve verbal memory in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 166 (7), 805-811.
Fiske, S.T., Taylor, S.E., 1991. Social cognition, 2nd ed. McGraw Hill, New York, NY.
Frommann, N., Streit, M., Wolwer, W., 2003. Remediation of facial affect recognition impairments in patients with schizophrenia: A new training program. Psychiatry Res. 117 (3), 281-284.
Gard, D.E., et al., 2006. Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. J. Res. Pers. 40,1086-1102.
Gard, D.E., Kring, AM., Gard, M.G., Horan, W.P., Green, M.F., 2007 Jull. Anhedonia in schizophrenia: distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophr. Res. 93 (1-3), 253-260.
Gard, D.E., Fisher, M., Garrett, C., Genevsky, A., Vinogradov, S., 2009. Motivation and its relationship to neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 115 (1), 74-81.
Green, M.F., Bearden, C.E., Cannon, T.D., Fiske, A.P., Hellemann, G.S., Horan, W.P., Kee, K., Kern, R.S., Lee, J., Sergi, M.J., Subotnik, K.L., Sugar, C.A., Ventura, J., Yee, C.M., Nuechterlein, K.H., 2012a. Social cognition in schizophrenia, Part 1: Performance across phase of illness. Schizophr. Bull. 38 (4), 854-864.
Green, M.F., Hellemann, G., Horan, W.P., Lee, J., Wynn, J.K., 2012b. From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: Modeling the role of ability and motivation. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69,1216-1224.
Green, M.F., Lee, J., Ochsner, K.N., 2013. Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 1: Ferrying paradigms across perilous waters. Schizophr. Bull. 39 (6), 1192-1200.
Gur, R.C., Ragland, J.D., Moberg, P.J., Bilker, W.B., Kohler, C., Siegel, S.J., Gur, R.E., 2001. Computerized neurocognitive scanning: II The profile of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 25 (5), 777-788.
Hooker, C., Parle, S., 2005. You must be looking at me: The nature of gaze perception in schizophrenia patients. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 10 (5), 327-345.
Hooker, C.I., Bruce, L., Fisher, M., Verosky, S.C., Miyakawa, A., Vinogradov, S., 2012 Augg. Neural activity during emotion recognition after combined cognitive plus social cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 139 (1-3), 53-59. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.009.
Hooker, C.I., Bruce, L., Fisher, M., Verosky, S.C., Miyakawa, A., D'Esposito, M., Vinogradov, S., 2013 Aug 30. The influence of combined cognitive plus social-cognitive training on amygdala response during face emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 213 (2), 99-107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.04.001.
Humphreys, L., Barrowclough, C., 2006. Attributional style, defensive functioning and persecutory delusions: Symptom-specific or general coping strategy? Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 45 (Pt 2), 231-246.
Insel, T.R., 2010. Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature 468 (7321), 187-193.
Irani, F., Seligman, S., Kamath, V., Kohler, C., Gur, R.C., 2012. A meta-analysis of emotion perception and functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 137 (1 -3), 203-211.
Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., Opler, L.A., 1987. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13 (2), 261-276.
Kern, R.S., Penn, D.L., Lee, J., Horan, W.P., Reise, S.P., Ochsner, K.N., Marder, S.R., Green, M.F., 2013. Adapting social neuroscience measures for schizophrenia clinical trials, part 2: Trolling the depths of psychometric properties. Schizophr. Bull. 39 (6), 1201 -1210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt127.
Kurtz, M.M., Richardson, C.L., 2012. Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: A meta-analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophr. Bull. 38 (5), 1092-1104.
Levitt, H., 1971. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 49(2), Suppl 2:467 +.
Lindenmayer, J.P., McGurk, S.R., Khan, A., Kaushik, S., Thanju, A., Hoffman, L., Valdez, G., Wance, D., Herrmann, E., 2013. Improving social cognition in schizophrenia: A pilot intervention combining computerized social cognition training with cognitive remediation. Schizophr. Bull. 39 (3), 507-517.
Marshall, M., Rathbone, J., 2011. Early intervention for psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 37 (6), 1111-1114.
Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D.R., Sitarenios, G., 2003. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion 3 (1), 97-105.
McGorry, P., 2011. Transition to adulthood: the critical period for pre-emptive, disease-modifying care for schizophrenia and related disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 37 (3), 524-530.
Merzenich, M.M., 2013. Soft-wired. Parnassus Publishing, San Francisco.
Mesholam-Gately, R.I., Giuliano, A.J., Goff, K.P., Faraone, S.V., Seidman, L.J., 2009. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 23 (3), 315-336.
Milev, P., Ho, B.C., Arndt, S., Andreasen, N.C., 2005. Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: A longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. Am. J. Psychiatry 162 (3), 495-506.
Nahum, M., Garrett, C., Powell, B., Poelke, G., Fisher, M., al., e., 2013a. Testing the Feasibility of a Novel Computerized Neuro-plasticity based training program to remediate Social Cognition Deficits in Schizophrenia ('SocialVille'), International Congress on Schizophrenia Research (ICOSR).
Nahum, M., Lee, H., Merzenich, M.M., 2013a. Principles of neuroplasticity-based rehabilitation. Prog. Brain Res. 207,141-171.
Penn, D., Roberts, D.L., Munt, E.D., Silverstein, E., Jones, N., Sheitman, B., 2005. A pilot study of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 80 (2-3), 357-359.
Pinkham, A.E., Harvey, P.D., 2013. Future directions for social cognitive interventions in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 39 (3), 499-500.
Pinkham, A.E., Penn, D.L., Green, M.F., Buck B., Healey, K., Harvey, P.D., 2013. The Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation Study: Results of the Expert Survey and RAND Panel. Schizophrenia bulletin.
Roberts, R.D., Schulze, R., O'Brien, K., MacCann, C., Reid, J., Maul, A., 2006. Exploring the validity of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) with established emotions measures. Emotion 6 (4), 663-669.
Rosse, R.B., Kendrick, K., Wyatt, R.J., Isaac, A., Deutsch, S.I., 1994. Gaze discrimination in patients with schizophrenia: Preliminary report. Am. J. Psychiatry 151 (6), 919-921.
Russ, J., 2008. Development of PROID, a computerized emotional prosody identification task. PennSci. J. 6 (2).
Russell, T.A., Chu, E., Phillips, M.L., 2006. A pilot study to investigate the effectiveness of emotion recognition remediation in schizophrenia using the micro-expression training tool. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 45 (Pt4), 579-583.
Sacks, S., Fisher, M., Garrett, C., Alexander, P., Holland, C., Rose, D., Hooker, C., Vinogradov, S., 2013. Combining computerized social cognitive training with neuroplasticity-based auditory training in schizophrenia. Clin. Schizophr. Relat. Psychoses 7 (2), 78A-86A
Sergi, M.J., Rassovsky, Y., Nuechterlein, K.H., Green, M.F., 2006. Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 (3), 448-454.
Sergi, M.J., Rassovsky, Y., Widmark, C., Reist, C., Erhart, S., Braff, D.L., Marder, S.R., Green, M.F., 2007. Social cognition in schizophrenia: relationships with neurocognition and negative symptoms. Schizophr. Res. 90 (1-3), 316-324.
Subramaniam, K., Luks, T.L., Fisher, M., Simpson, G.V., Nagarajan, S., Vinogradov, S., 2012. Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia. Neuron 73 (4), 842-853.
Tso, I.F., Carp, J., Taylor, S.F., Deldin, P.J., 2013. Role of visual integration in gaze perception and emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia bulletin.
Tso, I.F., Mui, M.L., Taylor, S.F., Deldin, P.J., 2012. Eye-contact perception in schizophrenia: relationship with symptoms and socioemotional functioning. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121 (3), 616-627.
Ventura, J., Wilson, S.A., Wood, R.C., Hellemann, G.S., 2013. Cognitive training at home in schizophrenia is feasible. Schizophr. Res. 143 (2-3), 397-398.
Wechsler, D., 2001. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading: WTAR Psychological Corporation.
Williams, L.M., Whitford, T.J., Flynn, G., Wong, W., Liddell, B.J., Silverstein, S., Galletly, C., Harris, A.W., Gordon, E., 2008. General and social cognition in first episode schizophrenia: identification of separable factors and prediction of functional outcome using the IntegNeuro test battery. Schizophr. Res. 99 (1 -3), 182-191.
Wolwer, W., Frommann, N., Halfmann, S., Piaszek, A., Streit, M., Gaebel, W., 2005. Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophr. Res. 80 (2-3), 295-303.
Wykes, T., Newton, E., Landau, S., Rice, C., Thompson, N., Frangou, S., 2007. Cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) for young early onset patients with schizophrenia: an exploratory randomized controlled trial. Schizophr. Res. 94 (1-3), 221-230.