Scholarly article on topic 'Social cognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and bipolar disorders with and without psychotic features'

Social cognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and bipolar disorders with and without psychotic features Academic research paper on "Clinical medicine"

CC BY-NC-ND
0
0
Share paper
Academic journal
Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
OECD Field of science
Keywords
{"Social cognition" / "Schizophrenia spectrum" / "Bipolar disorder" / "Psychotic features" / MATRICS}

Abstract of research paper on Clinical medicine, author of scientific article — George C. Nitzburg, Katherine E. Burdick, Anil K. Malhotra, Pamela DeRosse

Abstract Background Social cognition may be critical to the impoverished social functioning seen in serious mental illness. However, although social-cognitive deficits are consistently demonstrated in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD), studies in bipolar disorder (BD) have produced inconsistent results. This inconsistency may relate to symptom profiles of patients studied, particularly the presence or absence of psychotic features. Thus, we examined social cognition in bipolar disorder with psychotic features (BD+) versus without psychotic features (BD−) relative to SSD and controls. Methods A sample of 537 SSD patients, 85 BD+ patients, 37 BD− patients, and 309 controls were administered the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, including a social cognition measure, the managing emotions branch of the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Analyses of covariance compared MSCEIT performance between diagnostic groups while controlling for race, psychotropic medication status, and neurocognition. Results SSD but not BD− or BD+ patients showed significant MSCEIT deficits relative to controls. Conclusions MSCEIT deficits were found in SSD but not BD− or BD+, suggesting that social cognition may represent an underlying difference between SSD and BD. However, variance in MSCEIT performance among BD patients may also suggest latent BD subgroups characterized by social-cognitive deficits. Findings can help inform future investigations into how social cognition and social brain development differ between SSD and BD.

Academic research paper on topic "Social cognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and bipolar disorders with and without psychotic features"

SCHIZOPHRENIA

ilUSEARCIh COGNITION

ELSEVIER

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Schizophrenia Research: Cognition

journal homepage: http://www.schizrescognition.com/

Social cognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and bipolar disorders with and without psychotic features^

George C. Nitzburg a,b* Katherine E. Burdickc,d, Anil K. Malhotra a,b,c, Pamela DeRosse a,b

a Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience, The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, Manhasset, NY 11030, USA

b Division of Psychiatry Research, The Zucker Hillside Hospital, Division of the North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System, Glen Oaks, NY, 11004, USA c Hofstra North Shore - LIJ School of Medicine, Departments of Psychiatry and Molecular Medicine, Hempstead, NY, 11550, USA d Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, 10029, USA

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Background: Social cognition may be critical to the impoverished social functioning seen in serious mental illness. However, although social-cognitive deficits are consistently demonstrated in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD), studies in bipolar disorder (BD) have produced inconsistent results. This inconsistency may relate to symptom profiles of patients studied, particularly the presence or absence of psychotic features. Thus, we examined social cognition in bipolar disorder with psychotic features (BD + ) versus without psychotic features (BD—)relative to SSD and controls.

Methods: A sample of 537 SSD patients, 85 BD + patients, 37 BD — patients, and 309 controls were administered the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, including a social cognition measure, the managing emotions branch of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Analyses of covariance compared MSCEIT performance between diagnostic groups while controlling for race, psychotropic medication status, and neurocognition. Results: SSD but not BD — or BD + patients showed significant MSCEIT deficits relative to controls. Conclusions: MSCEIT deficits were found in SSD but not BD — or BD+, suggesting that social cognition may represent an underlying difference between SSD and BD. However, variance in MSCEIT performance among BD patients may also suggest latent BD subgroups characterized by social-cognitive deficits. Findings can help inform future investigations into how social cognition and social brain development differ between SSD and BD. © 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license

(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

CrossMark

Article history:

Received 10 September 2014

Received in revised form 12 December 2014

Accepted 17 December 2014

Available online 2 February 2015

Keywords: Social cognition Schizophrenia spectrum Bipolar disorder Psychotic features MATRICS

1. Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) patients suffer from multiple functional impairments including impoverished social functioning (Cannon et al., 1997; Pope et al., 2007), which have prompted investigations into social-cognitive deficits in BD. Social cognition is a multidimensional construct that includes identifying affect others display, inferring others' mental states, attributing meanings to others' actions, reading others' social cues, applying a working knowledge of social rules to socially reciprocate in kind, and managing one's own emotional responses to maintain interpersonal relationships (Green et al., 2008a; Green and

☆ Funding/Support: This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health to Dr. DeRosse (MH086756), Dr. Burdick (MH100125), and Dr. Malhotra (MH079800); the NSLIJ Research Institute General Clinical Research Center (M01 RR018535); Advanced Center for Intervention and Services Research (P30 MH090590); and a Center for Intervention Development and Applied Research (P50 MH080173). Declaration of interests: None.

* Corresponding author at: North-Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System, The Zucker Hillside Hospital, 75-59 263rd Street, Glen Oaks, NY 11004, USA. Tel.: +1 718 470 8162; fax: +1 718 3431659.

E-mail address: gnitzburg@nshs.edu (G.C. Nitzburg).

Horan, 2010; Pinkham et al., 2014). Understanding bipolar patients' social-cognitive deficits may help illuminate mechanisms underlying impaired social competence in BD, which may help inform interventions to improve psychosocial functioning.

Yet to date, results of social cognition studies in BD have been inconsistent. While some studies have found social-cognitive deficits (Bora et al., 2005; Bozikas et al., 2006; David et al., 2014; Harmer et al., 2002; Hoertnagl et al., 2011; Inoue et al., 2004; Kerr et al., 2003; Lembke and Ketter, 2002; Martino et al., 2008, 2011; Soeiro-de-Souza et al., 2012a,b; Wolf et al., 2010), others have found mixed results with preserved aspects alongside deficits (Addington and Addington, 1998; Getz et al., 2003; McKinnon et al., 2010; Montag et al., 2010; Olley et al., 2005; Schaefer et al., 2010; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Summers et al., 2006; Vederman et al., 2012), and others have found no deficits relative to controls (Barrera et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Van Rheenen and Rossell, 2014; Vaskinn et al., 2007; Venn et al., 2004). Social-cognitive differences have also been found between BD—I and BD—II, or between manic or depressive phases of BD (Derntl et al., 2009; Gray et al., 2006). As a result, the nature of social-cognitive deficits in BD remains inconclusive.

In contrast, schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) have consistently shown substantial deficits in both social cognition and

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2014.12.003

2215-0013/© 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

neurocognition (Corbera et al., 2013; Dawson et al., 2012; Eack et al., 2010; Fiszdon et al., 2013; Green et al., 2012; Kee et al., 2009; Kern et al., 2011; Kimhy et al., 2012; Thompson et al., 2012; Torgalsb0en et al., 2014; Tso et al., 2010). Findings suggest that such deficits remain stable across phases of psychosis (Green et al., 2012) and are linked to SSD patients' quality of life and community, work, and social functioning (Fiszdon et al., 2013; Horan et al., 2012; Kee et al., 2009; Maat et al., 2012; Shamsi et al., 2011; Shi et al., 2013; Tso et al., 2010). Moreover, results suggest that social cognition is more strongly linked with community functioning than neurocognition (Fett et al., 2011), and mediates the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcomes (Bell et al.,2009; Schmidt et al., 2011).

Recently, it has been suggested that inconsistent past findings in BD may reflect the impact of co-occurring psychotic symptoms on social-cognitive performance (Thaler et al., 2013a,b), especially considering ~60% of bipolar patients will also experience psychotic symptoms at some point during their lifetimes (Dunayevich and Keck, 2000). Specifically, bipolar patients with psychotic features (BD + ) have shown greater social-cognitive deficits than those without psychotic features (BD —) (Thaler et al., 2013a,b). Although we previously reported (Burdick et al., 2011) no significant effect of a history of psychotic features in a bipolar sample substantially overlapping with the sample used in our current analyses, our prior work did not control for neurocognition and did not directly compare against SSD, which may have limited these past findings. In addition, although a study by Lee et al. (2013) also found no effect of a history of psychotic features on their BD patients' social-cognitive deficits, only 15 of their 68 patients (~22%) had a history of psychotic features, and thus this sample may have been underpowered to detect group differences. Therefore, it is possible that social-cognitive deficits may present in BD + but not BD — when directly compared to SSD and controls.

Examining social cognition across SSD, BD+, BD —, and controls may illuminate whether a history of psychotic features impairs social cognition in BD relative to SSD. Findings could help identify BD patients in need of social-cognitive interventions and help elucidate if social cognition represents an underlying difference between BD and SSD. Thus, we compared

social-cognitive performance differences among these diagnoses while controlling for neurocognition and psychotropic medication status.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Our sample consisted of 968 participants, with 537 SSD patients (447 schizophrenia and 90 schizoaffective disorder patients), 85 BD + patients, 37 BD — patients, and 309 controls. For full demographics and clinical characteristics, see Table 1. All participants gave written informed consent to protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of the North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System (NSLIJ). Controls were recruited using newspaper and Internet advertisements, posted flyers, and personal referrals. Patients were referred from the outpatient and inpatient psychiatric departments of Zucker Hillside Hospital (ZHH), part of NSLIJ. Controls were excluded if diagnosed with past or present Axis-I disorders, had substance abuse within the past month, or had a history of learning disability, neurological disorder, or CNS trauma. Patients were excluded if they had a history of learning disability, neurological disorder, or CNS trauma.

2.2. Clinical assessments

2.2.1. Diagnostic assessment

Controls were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV, non-patient edition (SCID-I/NP; First et al., 2001a) and patients were administered the patient edition (SCID-I/P; First et al., 2001b) by Ph.D. or Master's level psychometricians. Controls' SCIDs were compiled into narrative case summaries and no Axis-I pathology was determined by consensus of two senior ZHH faculty. Patients' SCIDs were supplemented by available records and compiled into narrative case summaries and diagnoses were determined by consensus of three senior ZHH faculty.

Table 1

Study group and subsample demographics and clinical characteristics.

Healthy control Bipolar disorder (BD) without Bipolar disorder (BD) with SSD spectrum disorder Statistic p

psychotic features Psychotic features

Total sample N 309 37 85 537

- BD 1 (N = 17) BD 2 (N = 20) BD 1 (N = 85) BD 2 (N = 0) - - -

Age: mean (SD) 40.52 (16.34) 41.24 (10.06) 40.61 (11.43) 43.26 (10.20) F(3,967) = 3.62 p < .05

% Female 55.99% 59.46%" 55.29% 26.82% X2(3,967) = 86.01 p < .001

% Caucasian 52.75% 54.05% 56.47% 37.80% X2(6,967) = 39.85 p < .001

Substance disorder history 0% 16.22% 27.05% 40.22% - -

Anticonvulsant use 0 18 32 186 - -

Antidepressant use 0 15 19 161 - -

Mood stabilizer use 0 11 21 40 - -

Antiparkinsonian use 0 4 10 139 - -

Anxiolytic use 0 5 8 56 - -

Antipsychotic use 0 23 54 461 - -

Sedative/Hypnotic use 0 4 6 26 - -

Stimulant use 0 1 0 6 - -

N of subsample with BPRS - 21 60 492 - -

- BD 1 (N = 11) BD 2 (N =10) BD 1 (N = 60) BD 2 (N = 0) - - -

Brief psychiatric rating - - - - - -

scale (BPRS): Mean (SD)

Thinking Disturbance - 3.86 (1.11) 4.23 (2.04) 6.16 (3.04) F(2,572) = 17.15 p < .001

Withdrawal/Retardation - 3.05 (1.07) 2.75 (1.28) 3.66 (1.86) F(2,572) = 7.86 p < .001

Hostile/Suspiciousness - 4.86 (1.68) 4.45 (1.78) 4.99 (2.06) F(2,572) = 1.92 ns

Anxious/Depression - 6.48 (2.89) 5.02 (2.40) 4.85 (2.22) F(2,572) = 5.22 p < .01

Activation - 4.86 (2.01) 4.47 (1.92) 4.47 (1.86) F(2,572) = 0.43 ns

BPRS Total Score - 29.10 (5.80) 27.07 (7.58) 31.14 (7.44) F(2,572) = 8.58 p < .001

2.3. Symptom assessment

Patients' clinical symptoms were assessed by trained Master's level psychometricians using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Overall and Gorham, 1962), a semi-structured clinical interview assessing 18 symptoms within the past week.

2.4. Cognitive assessment

To assess neurocognition, all participants were administered the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB), which consists of 10 measures assessing seven domains: (1) processing speed, measured by the symbol coding subtest of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS symbol coding; Keefe et al., 2004), the Trail Making Test: Part A (Trails-A; Army Individual Test Battery, 1944), and Category Fluency: Animal Naming (animal naming; Spreen and Strauss, 1998); (2) attention/vigilance, measured by the Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs (CPT-IP; Cornblatt et al., 1988); (3) working memory, measured by the spatial span subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition (WMS-III spatial span; Wechsler, 1997) and Letter-Number Span (LNS; Gold et al., 1997); (4) verbal learning, measured by the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt and Benedict, 2001); (5) visual learning, measured by the Brief VisuospatialMemory Test—Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997); (6) reasoning and problem solving, measured by the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery mazes subtest (NAB Mazes; Stern and White, 2003); and (7) social cognition, measured by the managing emotions branch of the Mayer-Salovey Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT; Mayer, 2002).

The MSCEIT has evidenced robust psychometric properties, with demonstrated applicability in BD (Burdick et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2013), SSD (Green et al., 2008b), and controls (Kern et al., 2008), good internal consistency (Eack et al., 2010; Mayer et al., 2003), good sensitivity to deficits in SSD and BD (Burdick et al., 2011; Kee et al., 2009; Van Rheenen and Rossell, 2014), and good validity in SSD, as SSD patients' MSCEIT performance has been significantly linked to social functioning (Shamsi et al., 2011) and residential independence (Lin et al., 2012), and has been improved by social-cognitive interventions (Eack et al., 2007).

2.5. MCCB scoring

While published norms were available for each MCCB domain (Kern et al., 2008), our sample of healthy control participants was better de-mographically matched to our bipolar and schizophrenia spectrum patient samples, as the Kern et al. (2008) data were not stratified by race. Specifically, the Kern et al. (2008) healthy control sample consisted of 76% Caucasian participants, whereas our healthy control participants consisted of 52.75% Caucasian participants, which was better matched to our BD—, BD +, and SSD patients (being 54.05%, 56.47% and 37.8% Caucasian, respectively). Thus, we chose to calculate Z-scores using means and standard deviations derived from our healthy control data, which provided improved demographic matching on race. Using our healthy control data, age and sex were regressed out of each

MCCB raw score to produce standardized residuals that were then transformed to Z-scores. A neurocognition score was calculated by averaging across the 6 nonsocial MCCB domains. Next, MSCEIT and neurocognition Z-scores were inspected for outliers over ± 3 standard deviations from the mean. There were no outliers for neurocognition, but 4 extreme low MSCEIT scores were identified and excluded (i.e. one BD + , two SSD, and one control case). Resulting age- and sex-corrected Z-scores consisted of (1) an MSCEIT Z-score and (2) a neurocognition Z-score. Both were normally distributed (K-S = 0.83 and 1.16 for neurocognition and MSCEIT, respectively, both p's > 0.05). Since we chose to derive MCCB scores using our own healthy control data, we have provided T-score means and standard deviations for all our diagnostic groups using the MCCB published norms in Table 2 for descriptive purposes and for ease of comparison to previous work on the MCCB.

2.6. Data analysis

Diagnostic groups were first compared on demographics using chi-squares for sex and race and analysis of variance for age. Demographic variables that significantly differed between groups were corrected for or included as covariates.

To investigate whether MSCEIT deficits would be demonstrated in all BD patients relative to SSD and controls, we first compared SSD patients, all BD patients, and controls using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for race, neurocognition, and psychotropic medication status. Next, to examine the impact of psychosis history on MSCEIT performance in BD, we conducted a follow-up ANCOVA comparing SSD, BD+, BD —, and control groups, controlling for race, neurocognition, and psychotropic medication status.

Race consisted of a categorical variable with Caucasian, African-American, and other minority group. Psychotropic medication status consisted of 8 dummy-coded variables that each assessed the presence or absence of anticonvulsant, antidepressant, mood stabilizer, antiparkinsonian, anxiolytic, antipsychotic, sedative, or stimulant use. All patients were on at least one form of psychotropic medication and thus all 8 variables were included in all ANCOVAs. No controls had any history of psychotropic medication use.

3. Results

Preliminary analyses revealed the sex and race distributions significantly differed between diagnostic categories, with fewer females and more minorities in the SSD group compared to other groups (sex: X2(3,967) = 86.01, p < 0.001; race: x2(6,967) = 39.85, p < 0.001). Age also significantly differed between groups, with SSD patients slightly older than controls but no significant age differences between any other groups (F (3,967) = 3.62,p < 0.05). Thus, we corrected all MCCB subtests for age and sex and controlled for race in all ANCOVAs.

Results from the first ANCOVA found significant MSCEIT differences between SSD patients, all BD patients, and controls (F (2,964) = 24.85, p < 0.001), controlling for race, neurocognition, and psychotropic

Table 2

MCCB domain scores by diagnostic group standardized using MCCB normative data.

MCCB domain Healthy control (N = 309) Bipolar disorder without psychotic features (N = 37) Bipolar disorder with psychotic features (N = 85) SSD spectrum disorder (N = 537)

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Processing speed 48.63 (10.43) 40.19 (10.69) 38.12 (9.05) 29.80 (14.03)

Attention vigilance 46.97 (10.66) 39.76 (12.91) 36.89 (12.86) 32.13 (12.36)

Working memory 44.82 (10.44) 37.57 (13.04) 37.47 (10.51) 31.76 (13.16)

Verbal learning 42.59 (8.28) 39.38 (8.96) 37.76 (7.29) 34.50 (7.36)

Visual learning 41.57 (11.74) 34.84(12.27) 35.80 (12.46) 30.31 (12.23)

Reasoning/problem solving 45.68 (10.38) 42.22 (8.72) 41.01 (8.75) 37.49 (8.33)

Social cognition 49.06 (10.98) 47.46 (11.34) 44.08 (13.50) 35.67 (12.05)

Overall composite score 42.68 (10.68) 34.00 (11.60) 31.67 (11.35) 22.55 (12.13)

medication status. Follow-up pairwise comparisons using Fisher's LSD found no significant MSCEIT differences between BD patients and controls, but rather only significant differences between SSD patients and controls and between BD and SSD patients. Significant covariates included neurocognition (F (1,964) = 92.29, p < 0.001) and antidepressant use (F (1,964) = 11.59, p < 0.001), while all other covariates were non-significant (all p's > 0.05).

Results from the second ANCOVA also found significant MSCEIT differences between the BD +, BD —, SSD, and control groups (F (3,964) = 16.83,p < 0.001), controlling for race, neurocognition, and psychotropic medication status. Follow-up pairwise comparisons using Fisher's LSD found no significant MSCEIT differences between BD +, BD —, and controls. Rather, the only significant difference was between SSD and all other groups. Significant covariates similarly included neurocognition (F (1,964) = 91.99, p < 0.001) and antidepressant use (F (1,964) = 11.02, p < 0.001), while all other covariates were non-significant (all p's > 0.05). For a plot of MSCEIT means across all four diagnostic groups, see Fig. 1.

4. Discussion

This study's primary aim was to examine social cognition across SSD, BD +, and BD — patients relative to controls. In particular, given ~60% of BD patients also experience psychotic symptoms at some point during their lifetimes, often during acute phases of illness (Dunayevich and Keck, 2000), and recent studies have found BD + patients to show significant social-cognitive deficits (Thaler et al., 2013a,b), our study aimed to elucidate whether BD + patients would show impaired MSCEIT performance relative to BD — patients and controls, as well as whether deficits would be consistent with SSD patients. However, in contrast to recent studies showing social-cognitive deficits in BD + (Thaler et al., 2013a,b), but consistent with past work showing no social-cognitive deficits in BD (Barrera et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Vaskinn et al., 2007; Venn et al., 2004), our analyses found significant

Fig. 1. Social cognition performance across diagnostic categories. Note: Error bars represent ± 1 standard error. MSCEIT = the managing emotions branch of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, the social cognition measure of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB). All MCCB scores including the MSCEIT are age- and sex-corrected, and the comparison of MSCEIT performance across diagnostic groupings controlled for race and neurocognition.

MSCEIT deficits in SSD but not BD + or BD — patients relative to controls. These results suggest that social cognition may represent a key underlying difference between SSD and BD.

Another possible explanation is that social-cognitive impairments of BD patients are more heterogeneous than those found in SSD. Consistent with this, our sample of BD + and BD — patients both had larger estimates of standard error on the MSCEIT than the other diagnostic groups, which might indicate the presence of latent bipolar subgroups that differ in their social-cognitive performance. To investigate this possibility, we examined a histogram of MSCEIT Z-scores for easily visible subgroups within our bipolar sample (both BD + and BD —). The distribution was unimodal and normally distributed (K-S = 0.83, p > 0.05), which was not suggestive of latent bipolar subgroups. It is still possible that there are latent subgroups of BD patients with different levels of social-cognitive performance, but that properly identifying these subgroups requires the use of more advanced statistical techniques such as hierarchical cluster analysis. Although outside the scope of the present study, future studies should use larger bipolar cohorts and more sophisticated analytic methods to investigate this possibility of latent subgroups of bipolar patients with differential social-cognitive performance.

An important limitation of our study was the lack of multiple measures of social cognition and the limited scope of the MSCEIT as social cognition measure. Specifically, the MSCEIT has evidenced some weaknesses in its ability to fully tap into the multifaceted construct of social cognition (Burdick et al., 2011), and Lee et al. (2013) used a battery that included multiple aspects of social cognition. Unfortunately, multiple social-cognitive measures were not available for our study. However, one benefit of the MSCEIT is that it is particularly well-suited to our study's transdiagnostic focus. Ceiling effects are a key problem for many social-cognitive measures, with controls often performing at over 90% accuracy (Dodell-Feder et al., 2013). As a result, past studies may have lacked sensitivity to subtle social-cognitive differences between BD patients and controls. In addition, the factor structure of social cognition in populations suffering from psychotic symptoms is still largely unknown and many measures have inadequate psychometric properties (Green et al., 2008a). In contrast, the MSCEIT was both developed for non-clinical populations and was the chosen consensus metric of the MCCB for studying serious psychiatric disorders (Kern et al., 2008; Nuechterlein et al., 2008), Thus, while limited in scope, the MSCEIT is apt for studying social-cognitive deficits across SSD and BD subtypes and for verifying past evidence of social-cognitive deficits in BD + patients (Lee et al., 2013; Thaler et al., 2013a,b). Nevertheless, social cognition remains a multifaceted construct that is not yet fully understood, and it is possible that BD patients may show deficits in social-cognitive domains not fully captured by the MSCEIT. Future studies are needed to develop and validate standardized, multifaceted social-cognitive batteries sensitive to subtle differences across clinical and nonclinical populations. Large-scale studies are also needed to assess performance on these batteries across all SSD and BD subtypes.

Our findings were also limited by differences in demographic distributions between study groups. Specifically, our SSD group was slightly older and had fewer females and more minorities compared to our other groups. However, we controlled for race in all analyses and MCCB subtests were all age- and sex-corrected, which helped attenuate any demographic differences between groups.

Although it may be argued that differences in symptom severity of our patient groups contributed to our findings, we ran a follow-up ANCOVA controlling for the total symptom severity score of the BPRS in a subsample of 570 patients who had been administered the BPRS (i.e., 21 BD—, 59 BD+, and 490 SSD patients). Results confirmed our findings, with no significant MSCEIT deficits in BD — or BD + but significant deficits in SSD relative to controls. Nevertheless, BPRS ratings were unfortunately not available for every patient and thus the possibility clinical symptoms impacted our findings cannot be completely ruled out. Moreover, differences in symptoms are often accompanied by differences in medication status. However, our analyses controlled for 8

classes of psychotropic medications and although including these 8 co-variates had the potential to adversely affect statistical power, our large sample size withstood the inclusion of these covariates while maintaining excellent power (1 — ¡3 = 0.9).

In summary, our evidence supports past work showing social-cognitive deficits in SSD but not BD + or BD — relative to controls (Barrera et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Vaskinn et al., 2007; Venn et al., 2004). While these data suggest that social cognition may represent an underlying difference between SSD and BD, the large variance in social-cognitive performance among the bipolar groups is also suggestive of heterogeneity in performance and may indicate a latent bipolar subgroup characterized by social-cognitive deficits. Results highlight the importance of developing standardized social-cognitive batteries for use across BD and SSD and emphasize the need for future work on social brain development in clinical populations.

Role and Funding Source

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health to Dr. DeRosse (MH086756), Dr. Burdick (MH100125), and Dr. Malhotra (MH079800); the NSLIJ Research Institute General Clinical Research Center (M01 RR018535); Advanced Center for Intervention and Services Research (P30 MH090590); and a Center for Intervention Development and Applied Research (P50 MH080173).

Contributors

Dr. Burdick, Dr. Malhotra, and Dr. DeRosse were responsible for protocol and study design and management of data collection. Dr. Nitzburg and Dr. DeRosse undertook the literature search and statistical analyses of the present study. All authors have contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

Acknowledgment

We thank Christopher Morell, M.A., for his help with data collection and Tracy Giordonello for her help with study coordination.

References

Addington, J., Addington, D., 1998. Facial affect recognition and information processing in

schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr. Res. 32 (3), 171-181. Army Individual Test Battery, 1944. Manual of directions and scoring. Barrera, Á., Vázquez, G., Tannenhaus, L., Lolich, M., Herbst, L., 2013. Theory of mind and functionality in bipolar patients with symptomatic remission. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment 6 (2), 67-74.

Bell, M., Tsang, H.W., Greig, T.C., Bryson, G.J., 2009. Neurocognition, social cognition, perceived social discomfort, and vocational outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 35 (4), 738-747.

Benedict, R.H.B., 1997. Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—Revised. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL. Bora, E., Vahip, S., Gonul, AS., et al., 2005. Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic

patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 112 (2), 110-116. Bozikas, V.P., Tonia, T., Fokas, K., Karavatos, A., Kosmidis, M.H., 2006. Impaired emotion processing in remitted patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 91 (1), 53-56. Brandt, J., Benedict, R.H., 2001. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised. Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. Burdick, K.E., Goldberg, T.E., Cornblatt, BA, etal., 2011. MATRICS consensus cognitive battery

in patients with bipolar I disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 36 (8), 1587-1592. Cannon, M., Jones, P., Gilvarry, et al., 1997. Premorbid social functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: similarities and differences. Am. J. Psychiatry 154 (11), 1544-1550.

Corbera, S., Wexler, B.E., Ikezawa, S., Bell, M.D., 2013. Factor structure of social cognition in schizophrenia: is empathy preserved? Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2013. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1155/2013/409205 (Article ID 409205).

Cornblatt, B.A., Risch, N.J., Faris, G., Friedman, D., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., 1988. The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 26, 223-238.

David, D.P., Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Moreno, R.A., Bio, D.S., 2014. Facial emotion recognition and its correlation with executive functions in bipolar I patients and healthy controls. J. Affect. Disord. 152,288-294.

Dawson, S., Kettler, L., Burton, C., Galletly, C., 2012. Do people with schizophrenia lack emotional intelligence? Schizophr. Res. Treat. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/ 495174 (Article ID 495174).

Derntl, B., Seidel, E.M., Kryspin-Exner, I., Hasmann, A., Dobmeier, M., 2009. Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 48 (4), 363-375.

Dodell-Feder, D., Lincoln, S.H., Coulson, J.P., Hooker, C.I., 2013. Using fiction to assess mental state understanding: a new task for assessing theory of mind in adults. PLoS One 8 (11), e81279.

Dunayevich, E.D., Keck, P.E., 2000. Prevalence and description of psychotic features in bipolar mania. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2 (4), 286-290.

Eack, S.M., Hogarty, G.E., Greenwald, D.P., Hogarty, S.S., Keshavan, M.S., 2007. Cognitive enhancement therapy improves emotional intelligence in early course schizophrenia: preliminary effects. Schizophr. Res. 89 (1), 308-311.

Eack, S.M., Greeno, C.G., Pogue-Geile, M.F., Newhill, C.E., Hogarty, G.E., Keshavan, M.S., 2010. Assessing social-cognitive deficits in schizophrenia with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Schizophr. Bull. 36 ( 2), 370-380.

Fett, A.K.J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M.D.G., Penn, D.L., van Os, J., Krabbendam, L., 2011. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35 (3), 573-588.

First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B., Gibbon, M., 2001a. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV—Non-Patient Edition, Version 1.0. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.

First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J.B., 2001 b. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition (SCID-I/P. 2/2001 Revision). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.

Fiszdon, J.M., Fanning, J.R., Johannesen, J.K., Bell, M.D., 2013. Social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and their relationship to clinical and functional status. Psychiatry Res. 205 (1), 25-29.

Getz, G.E., Shear, P.K., Strakowski, S.M., 2003. Facial affect recognition deficits in bipolar disorder. JINS 9 (04), 623-632.

Gold, J.M., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T.E., Weinberger, D.R., 1997. Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54,159.

Gray, J., Venn, H., Montagne, B., et al., 2006. Bipolar patients show mood-congruent biases in sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion when exhibiting depressed symptoms, but not when exhibiting manic symptoms. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 11 (6), 505-520.

Green, M.F., Horan, W.P., 2010. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19 (4), 243-248.

Green, M.F., Penn, D.L., Bentall, R., et al., 2008a. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr. Bull. 34(6), 1211-1220.

Green, M.F., Nuechterlein, K., Kern, R., et al., 2008b. Functional co-primary measures for clinical trials in schizophrenia: results from the MATRICS Psychometric and Standardization Study. Am. J. Psychiatry 165 (2), 221-228.

Green, M.F., Bearden, C.E., Cannon, T.D., et al., 2012. Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 1: performance across phase of illness. Schizophr. Bull. 38 (4), 854.

Harmer, C.J., Grayson, L., Goodwin, G.M., 2002. Enhanced recognition of disgust in bipolar illness. Biol. Psychiatry 51 (4), 298-304.

Hoertnagl, C.M., Muehlbacher, M., Biedermann, F., et al., 2011. Facial emotion recognition and its relationship to subjective and functional outcomes in remitted patients with bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord. 13 (5-6), 537-544.

Horan, W.P., Green, M.F., DeGroot, M., et al., 2012. Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 2: 12-month stability and prediction of functional outcome in first-episode patients. Schizophr. Bull. 38 (4), 865.

Inoue, Y., Tonooka, Y., Yamada, K., Kanba, S., 2004. Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 82 (3), 403-409.

Kee, K.S., Horan, W.P., Salovey, P., et al., 2009. Emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 107 (1), 61-68.

Keefe, R.S., Goldberg, T.E., Harvey, P.D., Gold, J.M., Poe, M.P., Coughenour, L., 2004. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr. Res. 68,283-297.

Kern, R.S., Nuechterlein, K., Green, M., et al., 2008. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization. Am. J. Psychiatry 165,214-220.

Kern, R.S., Gold, J.M., Dickinson, et al., 2011. The MCCB impairment profile for schizophrenia outpatients: results from the MATRICS psychometric and standardization study. Schizophr. Res. 126 (1), 124-131.

Kerr, N., Dunbar, R.I., Bentall, R.P., 2003. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar affective disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 73 (3), 253-259.

Kimhy, D., Vakhrusheva, J., Jobson-Ahmed, L., Tarrier, N., Malaspina, D., Gross, J.J., 2012. Emotion awareness and regulation in individuals with schizophrenia: implications for social functioning. Psychiatry Res. 200 (2), 193-201.

Lee, J., Altshuler, L., Glahn, D.C., Miklowitz, D.J., Ochsner, K., Green, M.F., 2013. Social and nonsocial cognition in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: relative levels of impairment. Am. J. Psychiatry 170 (3), 334-341.

Lembke, A., Ketter, T.A., 2002. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. Am. J. Psychiatry 159 (2), 302-304.

Lin, Y., Wynn, J.K., Hellemann, G., Green, M.F., 2012. Factor structure of emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 139 (1-3), 78-81.

Maat, A., Fett, A.K., Dermis, E., 2012. Social cognition and quality of life in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 137 (1), 212-218.

Martino, D.J., Strejilevich, S.A., Scapola, M., et al., 2008. Heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 109 (1), 149-156.

Martino, D.J., Strejilevich, S.A., Fassi, G., Marengo, E., Igoa, A., 2011. Theory of mind and facial emotion recognition in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Psychiatry Res. 189 (3), 379-384.

Mayer, J.D., 2002. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEITTM) (North Tonawanda, NY).

Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D.R., Sitarenios, G., 2003. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion 3 (1), 97.

McKinnon, M.C., Cusi, A.M., MacQueen, G.M., 2010. Impaired theory of mind performance in patients with recurrent bipolar disorder: moderating effect of cognitive load. Psychiatry Res. 177 (1), 261-262.

Montag, C., Ehrlich, A., Neuhaus, K., et al., 2010. Theory of mind impairments in euthymic bipolar patients. J. Affect. Disord. 123 (1), 264-269.

Nuechterlein, K., Green, M., Kern, R., et al., 2008. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 165 (2), 203-213.

Olley, A.L., Malhi, G.S., Bachelor, J., Cahill, C.M., Mitchell, P.B., Berk, M., 2005. Executive functioning and theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 7 (s5), 43-52.

Overall, J.E., Gorham, D.R., 1962. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol. Rep. 10 (3), 799-812.

Pinkham, A.E., Penn, D.L., Green, M.F., Buck, B., Healey, K., Harvey, P.D., 2014. The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr. Bull. 40 (4), 813-823.

Pope, M., Dudley, R., Scott, J., 2007. Determinants of social functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 9 (1-2), 38-44.

Schaefer, K.L., Baumann, J., Rich, BA, Luckenbaugh, DA., Zarate Jr., C.A., 2010. Perception of facial emotion in adults with bipolar or unipolar depression and controls. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44 (16), 1229-1235.

Schmidt, S.J., Mueller, D.R., Roder, V., 2011. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr. Bull. 37 (Suppl. 2), S41-S54.

Shamay-Tsoory, S., Harari, H., Szepsenwol, O., Levkovitz, Y., 2009. Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 21 (1), 59-67.

Shamsi, S., Lau, A., Lencz, T., et al., 2011. Cognitive and symptomatic predictors of functional disability in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 126 (1), 257-264.

Shi, C., He, Y., Cheung, E.F., Yu, X., Chan, R.C., 2013. An ecologically valid performance-based social functioning assessment battery for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 210 (3), 787-793.

Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Bio, D.S., David, D.P., 2012a. COMT Met (158) modulates facial emotion recognition in bipolar I disorder mood episodes. J. Affect. Disord. 136 (3), 370-376.

Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Otaduy, M.C.G., Dias, C.Z., Bio, D.S., Machado-Vieira, R., Moreno, R.A., 2012b. The impact of the CACNA1C risk allele on limbic structures and facial emotions recognition in bipolar disorder subjects and healthy controls. J. Affect. Disord. 141 (1), 94-101.

Spreen, O., Strauss, E., 1998. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 2nd edition. Oxford University Press, New York NY.

Stern, RA, White, T., 2003. Neuropsychological assessment battery (NAB). Psychological Assessment Resources, Lutz, FL.

Summers, M., Papadopoulou, K., Bruno, S., Cipolotti, L., Ron, M.A., 2006. Bipolar I and bipolar II disorder: cognition and emotion processing. Psychol. Med. 36 (12), 1799-1809.

Thaler, N.S., Strauss, G.P., Sutton, G.P., Vertinski, M., Ringdahl, E.N., Snyder, J.S., Allen, D.N., 2013a. Emotion perception abnormalities across sensory modalities in bipolar disorder with psychotic features and schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 147 (2), 287-292.

Thaler, N.S., Allen, D.N., Sutton, G.P., Vertinski, M., Ringdahl, E.N., 2013b. Differential impairment of social cognition factors in bipolar disorder with and without psychotic features and schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47 (12), 2004-2010.

Thompson, A., Papas, A., Bartholomeusz, C., et al., 2012. Social cognition in clinical "at risk" for psychosis and first episode psychosis populations. Schizophr. Res. 141 (2), 204-209.

Torgalsboen, A.K., Mohn, C., Rishovd Rund, B., 2014. Neurocognitive predictors of remission of symptoms and social and role functioning in the early course of firstepisode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 216 (1), 1 -5.

Tso, I.F., Grove, T.B., Taylor, S.F., 2010. Emotional experience predicts social adjustment independent of neurocognition and social cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 122 (1), 156-163.

Van Rheenen, T.E., Rossell, S.L., 2014. An empirical evaluation of the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 16 (3), 318.

Vaskinn, A., Sundet, K., Friis, S., et al., 2007. The effect of gender on emotion perception in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 116 (4), 263-270.

Vederman, A.C., Weisenbach, S.L., Rapport, L.J., et al., 2012. Modality-specific alterations in the perception of emotional stimuli in bipolar disorder compared to healthy controls and major depressive disorder. Cortex 48 (8), 1027-1034.

Venn, H.R., Gray, J.M., Montagne, B., et al., 2004. Perception of facial expressions of emotion in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 6 (4), 286-293.

Wechsler, D., 1997. WMS-III: Wechsler memory scale administration and scoring manual. The Psychological Corporation, London, UK.

Wolf, F., Brüne, M., Assion, H.J., 2010. Theory of mind and neurocognitive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 12 (6), 657-666.